COUNSELING CENTER
UNIVERSITY OF MARYLAND
COLLEGE PARK, MARYLAND
ATTITUDES OF YOUNG ADULTS
TOWARD CHILDREN
Patricia T. Carney and
William E. Sedlacek
Research Report # 4-85
Computer time for this study was furnished by the Computer
Science Center, University of Maryland, College Park, Md.
COUNSELING CENTER
UNIVERSITY OF MARYLAND
COLLEGE PARK, MARYLAND
Patricia T. Carney and
William E. Sedlacek
Research Report # 4-85
SUMMARY
Children hold a special
place in society. They are lavished with care, but are also the targets of many
forms of discrimination. This study assessed the attitudes toward children of
246 freshmen entering the University of Maryland, College Park, using the
Situational Attitude Scale - Adults/Children (SAS-A/C).
Multivariate analyses of variance (sex by form) indicated that students tended
to react more negatively to a variety of personal and social situations when
children under the art: of seven were included in those situations. There were
few differences by sex or the social distance involved in the situations.
Implications for the education and development of young adults, and the ways in
which they may have learned such prejudice are discussed.
Children
hold a special place in society. They are lavished with care, admired, and
spoiled. Millions of: dollars are spent each year on toys and games to
entertain them. There are more childrearing books on the market today than ever
before, reflecting our concern for the physical and mental well-being of
our children (Klausner, 1968). Children are a cherished group: it seems almost
impossible to think that children may be subject to the same stereotypes,
prejudice and discrimination as any other group. And yet, children are
restricted from participating in the very decisions that affect their personal,
social, and economic lives (Goodman, 1964). Formal laws, as well as the
informal rules and traditions of the community and the family all seek to
restrict the autonomy of children. Holt (1974) has noted that children are
banned from at least one fourth of all rental housing as well as a variety of
other forms of residential facilities. According to the Children's Defense Fund
(1983), children were among the first to feel the impact of recent federal budget
cuts. Even more tragic is the fact that millions of children are the victims of
child abuse every year (Genes, 1979). Such phenomena have led to the creation.
of forces such as the child advocacy movement (Margolin, 1978), yet little
2
scientific research has been attempted to understand
the antecedents of such behavior.
The literature contains
research on prejudicial attitudes toward a number of identified groups
including blacks (e.g., Minatoya, Sedlacek, & Brooks, 1984), women (e.g.,
Minatoya & Sedlacek, 1983), and the disabled (e.g., Stovall & Sedlacek,
1983). In recent years, age has become a category for consideration in this
body of literature. In 1971, McTaVish presented a review of the literature on
perceptions of old people. According to his findings, old people are generally
viewed as ill, tired, mentally slower, not sexual, forgetful, withdrawn,
unproductive, grouchy and defensive. Research has also shown an age bias among
college students toward older students (Peabody & Sedlacek, 1382); and of
educational and college student personnel professionals toward older adults
(Celio, Sedlacek, & Schlossberg, 1977). Riley, Johnson and Foner (1972)
have suggested that a cohort-centrism may exist and that individuals
express negative attitudes toward others of different age groups. If this is
the case, such negative attitudes may also extend toward children. There are
two good reasons to study (negative) attitudes toward children. First, research
has not yet established whether or not such prejudicial attitudes and negative
affects actually do
3
exist. An exploration and acknowledgement of these
attitudes may help us comprehend what currently appears to be arbitrary
discrimination toward children. Second, research shows that attitudes toward an
individual or group can at least predict the overall pattern of behavior toward
that individual or group. Racist, sexism and other forts of prejudice have
historically resulted in a variety of discriminatory actions ranging from
avoidance and exclusion to outright violence. Ashmore's (1970) definition of a
minority group states: "The key point about a minority group is that it is
in a subordinate position with regard to status and power" (p.250).
Children are a minority group by this definition and may be subject to the same
negative cognitions, affects and discriminatory behaviors as other groups..
Attitudes are a good place to begin to explore this pattern.
As was stated earlier, very
little research has actually been done on the attitudes of adults toward
children. What little research exists, has usually focused on specific adult-child
interactions such as attitudes toward child rearing (e.g. Cohen & Eiduson,
1973), attitudes of teachers toward their students (e.g. Khan & Weiss,
1978), and attitudes of the elderly toward children (e.g. Cryns & Monk,
1972; Higgins & Faunce, 1977; Seefeldt & Jantz, 1979). A
4
more general study was conducted by Rowers and
Wrightsman (1978) in which they developed scales to measure a respondent's
orientation toward children's rights. The scales contained two polar ends:
nurturance (giving the child what you believe is good for him/her) and self-determination
(allowing the child to make his/her own decisions). The scales were
administered to high school juniors and seniors, undergraduate college
students, and adults in continuing education programs. In all cases,
respondents were more 1. likely to favor the nurturant poles of the scales at
the expense of the child's self-determination. Bohrnstedt, Freeman and
Smith (1981.) conducted a more thorough study o£ adult attitudes toward
children's autonomy. The study included 1,002 adults responding to vignettes on
parent-child conflict. Their results indicated that an individual is a
more likely to take the side of the child in a conflict if the respondent Is
young, well-educated, has no religious identification or has a Jewish
background, is Anglo-white or Asian, and the child in question is older
(i.e. teenager). An Individual is more likely to take the side of the parent in
a conflict if the respondent is older, less well-educated, has a Catholic
or Protestant background, is black or Hispanic, and the child in question is
younger (i.e. pre-teen., early teens).
5
Knight, Seefeldt, and
Sedlacek (1.984) attempted a more general investigation into the attitudes of
adults toward children under the age of 12. A Situational Attitude Scale-Adults/Children
(SAS-A/C) was developed to assess whether attitudes toward children in
social situations are positive, negative, or neutral. The SAS methodology was
originally developed to measure the degree of prejudice one racial group holds
toward another (Sedlacek & Brooks, 1974). The methodology has been
successfully applied to a number of different racial groups as well as to sex
(Minatoya and Sedlacek, 1983), age (Peabody & Sedlacek, 1982), and disabled
individuals (Stovall and Sedlacek, 1983). The methodology presents subjects
with ten social, situations or vignettes which reflect varying degrees of
social distance and asks them to respond to ten bipolar pairs of adjectives for
each situation. These adjectives reflect three dimensions: evaluative, potency,
and activity. Half the subjects receive a version to which children are present
in each of the ten social situations while the other half receive a version in
which no mention of children is made. Research indicates that this procedure
limits psychological withdrawal and makes it more difficult for subjects to
respond on the basis of social desirability (Sedlacek & Brooks, 1972). The
results of the Knight et al. (1984)
6
study showed that in nine of the ten social
situations, attitudes were significantly different when children were present.
Situations representing close social proximity were most likely to be viewed as
negative along the evaluative dimension. There were no significant sex
differences.
The current study was
conceived of as a substantive as well as a methodological inquiry into the
attitudes of adults toward children. It is basically a replication and an
extension oaf Knight et al (1984) and attempts to address several questions
left unanswered by that study. First, is, the SAS -A/C an appropriate tool for
the study of attitudes toward children; are the results replicable? Second, can
a shortened version of the SAS-A/C be utilized thus reducing the number
of items from 100 to 50? Third, the Knight et al. study used a young adult
population. Would the same results be found with a younger population? When do
these attitudes toward children develop? And fourth, the Knight et al. study
utilized only univariate analyses. Multivariate approach may be more
appropriate for the complex data generated by the SAS-A/C. It was
hypothesized that the results of this study would parallel and therefore
reinforce the results of the previous research: situations including children
would be viewed more negatively than
7
situations in which no mention of children is made.
A younger population was utilized and a multivariate analysis of variance
(MANOVA) was employed to reflect more accurately the multivariate nature of the
data. This study also sought to investigate how attitudes toward children vary
with the degree of social distance and whether or not there are any sex
differences in attitudes toward children.
Subjects
Subjects for this study were
246 entering freshmen (43% male, 57% female, 7% black, 74% white, 5% Asian, 1%
Hispanic) at a large eastern university. Due to missing data, not all subjects
were included in all analyses. The modal age was 18 with only 2% of the
subjects under the age of 17 and 1% over the
age of 19. The majority of subjects (86%) had fathers in professional.
or semi-professional occupations; 77% also indicated that their mother
was employed either full-time or part-time, the majority of these
women (64%) in professional or semi-professional occupations. Twenty-nine
percent identified themselves as Catholic, 31% as Jewish, 24% as Protestant, 8%
as other, and 8% as none.
8
A shortened version of the
Situational Attitude Scale-Adults/Chilren (SAS-AC) was utilized for this
study. The SAS methodology was originally developed to measure the degree of
prejudice one racial group holds toward another (Sedlacek Brooks, 1970).
Development of the SAS-AC is described in detail in Knight, Seefeldt and
Sedlacek (1980). Briefly, the instrument used in the present study consisted of
five hypothetical situations representing previously measured levels of social
distance. The five situations in orders of increasing social-distance
included: 1) having a person as a guest in my home for a week; 2) renting a
room to someone in my home; 3) having a person I live in the apartment next to
mine 4) having dinner in a nice restaurant with another person sitting nearby;
and 5) sitting next to a person on an airplane flying non-stop from
Washington, DC to San Francisco. Each situation was followed by ten bipolar
scales representing the three dimensions of semantic meaning: evaluation,
potency, and activity. The adjectives used for the bipolar scales were
suggested by Osgood, Suci, and Tannenbaum (1957). There were two forms of the
SIBS-AC. The control form did not designate the ages of any of the
participants in the situation. In the experimental form, however, a child or
9
children under the age of seven was included. Both forms consisted of identical instructions, social situations and bipolar scales. The instructions and situations are included in Exhibit 1.
The two forms of the SAS-AC
were randomly ordered and administered simultaneously on each occasion they
were administered. Students were not aware there was more than one form of the
scale. The administrator, an adult white female, read the directions to the
group and was assisted by several student workers. Participation was voluntary
and anonymous and all students present at the sessions on the selected days
participated. Administration required less than 15 minutes.
A multivariate analysis of
variance (MANOVA) was performed over the complete, 50-item SAS-AC
questionnaire with sex (male vs. female) and form (children vs. no children) as
the grouping variables. Results from this analysis were significant for form,
(50,157)= .532, p<.01. Twenty-nine of the 50 univariate analyses
performed an the data were also significant for form at tae p<.01 level.
(This is a larger number of significant analyses than would
10
be expected by chance; Sakoda, Cohen, a Beall,
1954). The means and standard deviations for these analyses are presented in
Table 1. In every case, situations involving children were viewed less
positively than situations not specifically including children. Overall, there
were no significant effects for sex (l (50, 157) = .800, p>.01
or for the interaction sex x form (l (50, 157) = .716, p>.01.
A separate two way MANOVA
was run on each of the five situations contained within the SAS-AC
questionnaire. A wins she overall Analysis form was significant for each of the
five situations at the p<.01 level. One of the situations also showed a
significant main effect for sex (p<.o1): males rated situation #1 more
negatively than did females Where were no significant interactions.
The results of thin study
show that subjects rated social situations more negatively when children under
the age of seven were included in those situations This result was consistent
across all situations regardless of the social distance involved. It was also
consistent across sex.. The only sex difference found was that males rated one
of the SAS situations more negatively overall (i.e., regardless of whether or
not children were present). This
11
situation (having dinner in a nice restaurant) was
one of the most socially distant situations on the SAS. It is
possible that females are socialized to be more
comfortable with such superficial types of social contact and so rated these
situations more favorably.
The SAS appears to be a
useful tool in the study of attitudes
toward children. Reducing the number of items from 100 to 50 appears to be an
economical move both in terms of the amount of time necessary to complete the
instrument and the number of subjects needed for statistical significance.
Further reductions, however may not be possible without losing the benefit of
including social situations of varying social distance. Although social
distance did not appear to be related to the attitudes toward children in this
study, it is still too early to eliminate this as a possible variable in future
research.
Further exploration into the
attitudes of adults toward children is needed at this point. At what age do
these attitudes develop? Such attitudes are already formed by the time an
individual reaches young adulthood as evidenced by this study and its
predecessor (Knight et al, 1984). Replication of this study with a younger
population would help answer this question. Does exposure have any effect on attitudes
toward children? Few if any of the subjects in
12
this study had children of their own or were exposed to children on a regular basis (the same was true in the Knight et al, 1384, study despite the older population). Repeating the shady with a sample of parents would prove informative on this point. The Bohrastedt et al (1981) study on the attitudes of adults toward children's autonomy found racial arid religious differences. Such an analysis on the current data would prove useful in .exploring some of the correlates of attitudes toward children. Finally, research investigating the relationship between attitudes and behavior toward children might prove very useful in understanding why society treats children as it does.
As we continue our study
into the attitudes of adults toward children, it is important to keep in mind
the broader social context. Even if ire show that negative attitudes, prejudice
and discrimination toward children exist, the analogy between, children and
other minority groups may necessarily be limited. As Bohrnstedt et al (1981)
point out: “Other disenfranchised groups usually have the capacity and
potential to compete equally with peers in. the community. Both experience and
scientific data indicate that mature judgment and the ability to make
individual decisions and to participate fully in the social life of the
community depend on age related cognitive and social
13
development” (p. 460). Child advocacy groups, including the Children's Defense Fund, acknowledge that children are, not adults and are in many ways dependent on adults for care. By the same token, children often do not have the resources available to them to protect themselves from the arbitrary behavior of adults. It is for this reason that we need to be particularly aware of negative attitudes and behaviors directed toward children. If children are unable to protect themselves, then the burden lies with the rest of us.
14
Ashmore,
R.D. Prejedice: causes and cures. In Social Psychology, by B.E.
Collins. Reading, Mass,: Addison-Wesley, 1970.
Bohrnstedt,
G.W., Freeman, H.E., and Smith, T. Adult perspectives on children's autonomy. Public
Opinion
Quarterly,
1981, 45(4), 443-462.
Celio,
M.B., Sedlacek, W.E., and Schlossberg, N.K. The development of a measure of
attitudes toward age. Counseling
Center Research Report #8-77, College Park, Md.: University of
Maryland, 1977.
Children's
Defense Fund. What is CDF? Questions and Answers about the Children's
Defense Fund. Washington, D.C.: Washington Research Project, 1978.
Children's
Defense Fund. A, children's defense
budget analysis of the presidents FY 1984 budget and children. Washington,
D.C.: Children's Defense Fund, 1983.
Cohen,
J. and Eiduson, B.T. Changing patterns of child rearing in alternative lifestyles:
Implications for
development.
In A. Davids (ed. ), Child personality
and psychopathology: Current topics. New York: John
Wiley,
1973.
Cryns,
A.G. and Monk, A. Attitudes of the aged toward the young: A multivariate study
in Intergenerational perception. Journal of Gerontology 1972, 107-112.
Gelles,
R.J. Family Violence Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, 1979.
Holt,
J. Escape from childhood. New York: Ballantine Press, 1974.
Khan,
S.B. & Weiss, J. The teachings of affective responses. In R.M.W. Travers
(Ed.) Second handbook of research on teaching. Chicago: Rand McNally,
1973.
K1ausne
r, S . Z . Two centuries of child-rearing manuals. Washington, D.C.: The Joint Commission on
Mental Health of Children, Inc., 1968.
Knight,
G.D., Seefeldt, C., and Sedlacek, W.E. Measuring the attitudes of adults toward
children. Counseling Center Research Report 4-84, College Park,
MD: University of Maryland, 1984.
Margolin,
C.R. Salvation as liberation: the movement for children's rights in a
historical context. Social Problems, 1979, 25, 441-452.
McTaVish,
D.G. Perceptions of old people: A review of research methods and findings. Gerotologist
11(4).
Part
2, 1971, 90-101.
16
Minatoya,
L.Y., and Sedlacek, W.E. THE SASW: A means to measure environmental
sexism. Journal of the National
Association for Women Deans, Administrators,
and Counselors, 1983, 47, 26-30.
Minatoya,
L.Y., Sedlacek, C., and Brooks, G.C. Assessing attitudes of white university
students toward blacks in a changing context. Journal of Non White Concerns
in Personnel and Guidance 1984, 12, 69-79.
Osgood,
C.E., Suci, G.J., and Tannenbaum, P.H. The measurement of meaning.
Urbana, Ill.: University of Illinois Press, 1957.
Peabody,
S.A. and Sedlacek, W.E. Attitudes of younger university students toward older
students. Journal of College Student Personnel. 1982, 23, 140-143.
Riley,
M.W., Johnson, M., and Foner, A. Aging and society: Vol. III: A sociology of
age stratification. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1972.
Rogers,
C.M. and Wrightsman, 1..a. Attitudes toward children's rights: nurturance or
self-determination? Journal of Social Issues, 34(2), 1978, 59-68.
Sakoda,
J.M., Cohen, B.H., and Beall, G. Tests of significance for a series of
statistical tests.
Psychological
Bulletin
51, 1954, 172-175.
17
Sedlacek,
W.E. and Brooks, G.C., Jr. The development of a measure of racial attitudes. Psychological
Reports, 27, 1970, 971-980.
Sedlacek,
W.E. and Brooks, G.C., Jr. Situational
Attitude Scale (SAS) Manual .Chicago: Natresources, Inc., 1972.
Seefeldt,
C. and Jantz, R.K. Elderly person's attitudes toward children. Unpublished
report. College Park, MD: University of Maryland, 1979.
Stovall,
C. and Sedlecek, W.E. Attitudes of sale and female university students toward
students with different physical disabilities. Journal of College Student
Personnel 1983, 325-338.
Table 1: Means*, Standard Deviations and Results of
Analyses of Variance |
|
|
||||||||
|
|
MALE |
FEMALE |
|
||||||
Item No. |
Situations** |
FORM A |
FORM B |
FORM A |
FORM B |
DIFFERENCES SIGNIFICANT AT .001 *** |
||||
|
Bipolar Adjective
Dimensions |
MEAN |
S.D. |
MEAN |
S.D. |
MEAN |
S.D. |
MEAN |
S.D. |
|
I. NICE RESTAURANT |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
1 |
friendly-unfriendly |
1.67 |
0.93 |
1 |
0.78 |
1.24 |
0.85 |
1.13 |
0.79 |
F |
2 |
passive-active |
1.73 |
0.96 |
1.64 |
1.06 |
1.87 |
1.01 |
1.69 |
0.89 |
|
3 |
tough-fragile |
1.85 |
0.74 |
1.7 |
0.63 |
2.06 |
0.83 |
2.05 |
0.74 |
S |
4 |
negative-positive |
2 |
1.05 |
2.59 |
0.84 |
2.35 |
1.03 |
2.84 |
0.93 |
F |
5 |
short-long |
1.81 |
0.87 |
2.07 |
0.76 |
1.96 |
0.89 |
2.03 |
0.64 |
|
6 |
deliberate-impulsive |
1.77 |
1.04 |
1.95 |
1.1 |
2.26 |
0.91 |
2.11 |
0.82 |
|
7 |
superior-inferior |
1.33 |
0.78 |
1.77 |
0.89 |
1.48 |
0.86 |
1.78 |
0.86 |
F |
8 |
weak-strong |
2.65 |
0.93 |
2.57 |
0.79 |
2.5 |
0.82 |
2.44 |
0.83 |
|
9 |
intentional-unintentional |
2 |
0.97 |
1.77 |
1.1 |
1.89 |
1.04 |
1.94 |
0.89 |
|
10 |
comfortable-uncomfortable |
1.94 |
1.24 |
1.05 |
1.08 |
1.39 |
1.27 |
1.2 |
1.1 |
F |
II. APARTMENT NEXT DOOR |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
11 |
fast-slow |
1.79 |
0.74 |
1.36 |
0.97 |
1.72 |
0.9 |
1.55 |
0.87 |
F |
12 |
humorous-serious |
1.63 |
1.1 |
1.2 |
0.9 |
1.48 |
0.93 |
1.38 |
0.85 |
|
13 |
willing-unwilling |
1.6 |
1.09 |
0.84 |
0.91 |
1.28 |
1.09 |
1 |
0.85 |
F |
14 |
bad-good |
2.27 |
0.89 |
2.91 |
0.91 |
2.46 |
1.04 |
2.89 |
0.91 |
F |
15 |
strong-weak |
1.52 |
0.97 |
1.3 |
0.85 |
1.46 |
0.82 |
1.44 |
0.73 |
|
16 |
active-passive |
1.56 |
1.09 |
1.23 |
0.89 |
1.39 |
0.96 |
1.09 |
0.83 |
F |
17 |
friendly-unfriendly |
1.06 |
0.86 |
0.43 |
0.59 |
1.06 |
1.16 |
0.5 |
0.71 |
F |
18 |
small-large |
2.27 |
1 |
2.32 |
0.8 |
2.04 |
0.91 |
2.2 |
0.78 |
|
19 |
hot-cold |
1.88 |
0.82 |
1.45 |
0.82 |
1.76 |
0.75 |
1.61 |
0.7 |
F |
20 |
unsociable-sociable |
2.6 |
1.16 |
3.27 |
0.9 |
3.07 |
1.15 |
3.33 |
0.86 |
F |
III. ROOM TO RENT |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
21 |
willing-unwilling |
2.04 |
1.38 |
0.89 |
0.89 |
2.13 |
1.39 |
1.14 |
0.99 |
F |
22 |
slow-fast |
2.12 |
0.94 |
2.2 |
0.79 |
2.18 |
0.97 |
2 |
0.85 |
|
23 |
weak-strong |
2.6 |
0.84 |
2.86 |
0.73 |
2.52 |
0.99 |
2.73 |
0.95 |
|
24 |
serious-humorous |
1.67 |
1.08 |
1.98 |
1.13 |
1.85 |
1.2 |
1.86 |
1.22 |
|
25 |
disapproving-approving |
1.85 |
1.2 |
2.25 |
0.94 |
2.02 |
1.21 |
2.38 |
0.83 |
F |
26 |
friendly-unfriendly |
1.33 |
1.02 |
0.7 |
0.82 |
0.64 |
0.72 |
1.11 |
0.96 |
F |
27 |
intentional-unintentional |
1.54 |
0.82 |
1.18 |
0.79 |
1.52 |
1 |
1.28 |
0.98 |
F |
28 |
small-large |
1.98 |
0.88 |
2.34 |
0.95 |
2.4 |
0.94 |
2.27 |
0.82 |
|
29 |
active-passive |
1.52 |
0.97 |
0.95 |
0.83 |
1.39 |
1.05 |
0.92 |
0.98 |
F |
30 |
comfortable-uncomfortable |
1.8 |
1.32 |
1.27 |
1.03 |
2.02 |
1.14 |
1.32 |
0.91 |
F |
IV. SEAT ON AIRPLANE |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
31 |
pleasurable – painful |
1.61 |
1.09 |
1.14 |
1.05 |
2.27 |
1.12 |
1.25 |
1.01 |
F, S |
32 |
simple-complex |
2 |
1.01 |
1.89 |
1.09 |
2.23 |
0.93 |
1.8 |
0.95 |
|
33 |
humorous-serious |
1.35 |
1.03 |
1.42 |
1.02 |
1.77 |
1.11 |
1.41 |
0.84 |
|
34 |
bad-good |
2.43 |
1.02 |
2.73 |
1.06 |
1.88 |
1.04 |
2.68 |
0.93 |
F |
35 |
short-long |
1.98 |
0.9 |
2.16 |
0.86 |
2.19 |
0.79 |
2.16 |
0.81 |
|
36 |
heavy-light |
2 |
1.03 |
2.2 |
0.99 |
2.02 |
0.91 |
2.45 |
0.76 |
F |
37 |
intentional-unintentional |
2 |
1.06 |
1.69 |
1.02 |
1.77 |
0.99 |
1.98 |
1.23 |
|
38 |
unwilling-willing |
2.33 |
1.13 |
2.66 |
0.91 |
2.02 |
1.1 |
2.7 |
0.9 |
F |
39 |
sociable-unsociable |
1.3 |
1.22 |
0.95 |
0.97 |
1.6 |
1.23 |
1 |
0.89 |
F |
40 |
active-passive |
1.5 |
1.08 |
1.5 |
1.08 |
1.88 |
1.06 |
1.41 |
0.9 |
|
V. HOUSE GUESTS |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
41 |
approving-disapproving |
1.63 |
1.3 |
0.72 |
0.95 |
2 |
1.15 |
0.73 |
1.11 |
F |
42 |
small-large |
2.13 |
1.06 |
2.5 |
0.98 |
2.44 |
0.92 |
2.41 |
0.97 |
|
43 |
unwilling-willing |
2.24 |
1.31 |
3.05 |
1.06 |
2.06 |
1.06 |
3.23 |
0.86 |
F |
44 |
weak-strung |
2.63 |
0.98 |
2.77 |
0.81 |
2.4 |
1 |
2.68 |
0.83 |
|
45 |
intentional-unintentional |
1.57 |
1.02 |
1.34 |
1.07 |
1.79 |
0.8 |
1.52 |
1.15 |
|
46 |
friendly-unfriendly |
1.15 |
1.19 |
0.45 |
0.78 |
1.23 |
1.04 |
0.5 |
0.95 |
F |
47 |
slow-fast |
2.2 |
1.02 |
2.58 |
0.87 |
2.31 |
0.75 |
2.48 |
1 |
F |
48 |
humorous-serious |
1.41 |
1.24 |
1.14 |
0.92 |
1.63 |
0.96 |
1.09 |
1.05 |
F |
49 |
uncomfortable-comfortable |
2.22 |
1.33 |
3.02 |
1 |
1.92 |
1.05 |
3.18 |
0.99 |
F |
50 |
active-passive |
1.26 |
1.08 |
0.8 |
0.96 |
1.67 |
1 |
1.05 |
1.12 |
F |
*
Scale A to E (numerical equivalent, 1-4)
**
See Exhibit 1 for complete situations
*** Results of 2-way analysis of
variance (fixed effects), with F (Form A or B) and S (Sex; male or female), as
main effects, and SxF as the interaction.