COUNSELING
CENTER
UNIVERSITY
OF MARYLAND
COLLEGE
PARK, MARYLAND
A
COMPARISON OF U.S. AND INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS
ON
ATTITUDES OF TOLERANCE FOR DIVERSITY
Marie L
. Miville, Bekele Molla,
and
William E. Sedlacek
Research
Report #7-92
Data were collected in cooperation with the Orientation
Office and were analyzed using facilities of the Computer Science Center, both
at the University of Maryland, College Park.
COUNSELING
CENTER
UNIVERSITY
OF MARYLAND
COLLEGE
PARK, MARYLAND
A
COMPARISON OF U.S. AND INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS
ON
ATTITUDES OF TOLERANCE FOR DIVERSITY
Research
Report #7-92
The current study examined intercultural attitudes and
behaviors of 246 incoming international students using the Cultural Orientation
and Awareness Test (Miville, Molla, & Sedlacek, 1992) . The results were
also compared with 131 incoming U.S. students. International students were
likelier to be older, male, and more extensively traveled than U.S. students.
These students additionally tended to hold more tolerant attitudes toward
diversity than U.S. students, although, overall, both groups of students were
found to be generally tolerant. International students were also more likely to
interact with persons of differing cultural groups than U. S. students.
These findings seem to imply that incoming international
students have high expectations toward the university setting and its ability
to respond to diversity as well as themselves and their ability to adapt to a
new and different environment. University programs aimed at facilitating
international student adaptation to the university setting may be built upon
these expectations, encouraging interaction among international students as a
group as well as with U.S. students. Such interaction can aid in the overall
development of both groups of students, particularly in maintaining and
strengthening initial attitudes of tolerance for diversity expressed by these
students.
1
While evidence indicates that the number of international
students entering US colleges has grown tremendously in recent decades (Boyan,
1981) , both researchers and student affairs professionals have been somewhat
slow in responding to this group's needs. Part of the problem stems from the heterogeneity
of the group; students who fall under the label "international
student" come from many countries and differ individually on a wide range
of problems, such as language proficiency and length of time in the U.S.
(Manese, Leong, & Sedlacek, 1985) . Such heterogeneity greatly challenges
student affairs professionals to effectively identify what are the needs of
international students, so that programming may be developed to address these
needs (Charles & Stewart, 1991) .
A lack of research interest and methodological problems
seem to have contributed to few studies focusing on international students.
Methodological issues have included the difficulty of accurately reflecting the
diversity of this group as well as the need to use a comparative group, for
example, U. S. students, to place the research in a context (Manese, Sedlacek,
& Leong, 1988) . Comparisons between international and US students can also
provide information on what common concerns and perceptions all students share,
as well as what differences there may be, socially and academically.
A number of issues have been identified as being important
to many international students. Some of these have financial, academic, and
professional themes, such as improving writing or speaking skills and obtaining
practical experience in their chosen fields (Manese, Leong, & Sedlacek,
1985) . Other concerns focus on personal adjustment, particularly feelings of
loneliness and social isolation that many international students experience
living in the United States.
2
One potential issue is the extent to which international
students expect to adjust well to the U.S., particularly to the people with
whom these students will be interacting. Problems in adjusting can negatively
affect academic performance of international students (Charles & Stewart,
1991) as well as increase feelings of alienation. This may lead a student to
leave school prematurely; research has already shown, for example, that making
on-campus connections is an important variable in the retention of Black
U.S. students (Sedlacek, 1987) .
Additionally, cultural barriers exist which may prevent
international students from adjusting to the new surroundings of a university
environment. These range from difficulties with English to lack of knowledge
about social norms in the U. S. (Brun & David, 1971; Manese, Leong, &
Sedlacek, 1985) . Attitudes of U.S. students themselves provide yet another
barrier to promoting international students' integration into campus life.
Miville, Molla, and Sedlacek (1992) found that U.S. students were somewhat
ambivalent in their attitudes toward people from diverse backgrounds. Although
some evidence exists to the contrary, international students have been found to
be aware of such ambivalence (Carter & Sedlacek, 1986). Discovering
international students' expectations of how well they think they will be
treated in the U. S. then may provide a partial portrait of the initial
adjustment problems these students may face.
The purpose of the current study is to compare intercultural
attitudes and behaviors of international students with U. S. students in order
to better understand both groups. Comparisons were also made between
international and U.S. students on attitudes of general tolerance toward people
from different backgrounds (eg., gays, persons with disabilities) - These
attitudes
3 were measured through the Cultural Orientation and
Awareness Test (COAT), an instrument developed by Miville, Molla, and Sedlacek (1992)
. The COAT assesses cognitive and behavioral components on intercultural
attitudes as well as a more general tolerance for diversity (eg., people with
disabilities) and attitudes toward homosexuality as originally designed by
Hansen (1982) . Method
The Cultural Orientation and Awareness Test (COAT) was
administered to 131 U.S. students (53% male, 47% female) and 246 international
students (70% male, 30% female) during an orientation program for entering
students at a large eastern university. The COAT is composed of 60 items
assessing both demographic and attitudinal variables. The data were summarized
through means, standard deviations,-and percentages. A MANOVA using type
of student (U.S. vs. international) as a main effect was conducted for items
assessing intercultural attitudes as well as general attitudes of tolerance.
The MANOVA involved a smaller sample of international students (n = 79) who
were attending orientation programming at the same time as U.S. students.
Results
Results are divided into the following themes: demographic
information, intercultural attitudes, general attitudes of tolerance, and
attitudes toward homosexuality.
Demographic information. Sixty-seven per
cent of international students surveyed were graduate students, while 33% were
undergraduate. In contrast, 100% of the U.S. students included in this study
were undergraduate. The racial/ethnic composition of the U.S. sample was: 86% White,
7% Black, 3% Asian-American, 3% Latino, and 0.08% American Indian. The
racial/ethnic makeup of the international sample was: 58% Asian, 21% White, 7% Latino,
6%
4 Black, and 8% Other. The mean age for international
students was 25 years while the mean age for U.S. students was 18 years. Forty-six
per cent of international students's parents had earned a four-year
degree or higher, while for 27% of these parents, the highest degree earned was
a high school diploma. Over seventy per cent of U . S . students' parents had
completed a four-year college degree or better.
In response to the question, "Have you lived in
a country other than your own," only 5% of U.S. students stated that they
had, while 30% of international students said they had lived in another
country. This latter percentage is surprisingly low, given that all of these
students were not U. S.
citizens. Fifty-nine per cent of international
students and 62% of U.S. students, however,
said they had traveled in
another country. Most U.S. students (74%) had traveled less than a month, while
the mean length of stay for international students was approximately five
months. Sixteen per cent of U.S. students and 32% of international students
stated that they had studied abroad.
When asked what had contributed most to their knowledge of
international affairs, both U.S. and international students identified news
organizations or the media (44% and 38%, respectively) as their primary source.
In another series of items, 62% of U.S. students and 76% of international
students stated that they watched television news daily or often . Sixty-eight
per cent of international students stated that they read a newspaper often or
daily as compared with 52% of U . S . students. Seventy-seven per cent of
international students also said that they read a news magazine often or
occasionally while 52% of U.S. students read news magazines occasionally (25% seldom
or never read a news magazine) - And 46% of U.S -students and 45%
of international
5 students listened to radio news daily or often .
Tables 1 through 3 summarize the means and standard deviations of the remaining COAT items for both US and international students.
Intercultural attitudes. Overall, international
students were significantly more likely on all intercultural behavior items to
be interactive (see Table 1) . That is, they were more likely than U . S .
students to have meals with someone from another country, attend social events
where people from different countries participate, invite people from another
country to visit, and obtain information about events outside the U.S.
Insert Table 1 about here
In contrast, U.S. and international students agreed on
most attitudinal items. Members of both groups stated that they were usually
comfortable interacting with people from other countries. Students from these
groups also tended to disagree with the statement, "Traveling abroad does
not usually help foster respect for other cultures." These students also
tended to disagree that cultural differences were so overwhelming that
conflicts among nations were inevitable .
Where there were significant differences between the
groups, international students tended to show the more positive attitude. For
example, these students tended to more strongly endorse the item,
"Traveling in other countries helps one appreciate different
cultures" than did U.S. students. International students also more strongly
disagreed with the item, "There is no other culture as good as my own.
" Finally, international students were more likely to prefer living with a
family when traveling in another country than
6
U.S. students. This contrasts, however, with the finding
that U.S. students found traveling more pleasurable when visiting places with a
group as opposed to oneself than did international students.
Attitudes of tolerance. As with intercultural
attitudes, U.S. and international students tended to agree on most items assessing
attitudes of tolerance. Again, where differences on individual items existed,
international students, for the most part, expressed the more tolerant attitude
(see Table 2) . Members from both groups, for example, tended to believe that
differences of opinions often lead to better solutions. Both groups of students
also tended to disagree with the item, "People from a different generation
often don't know what is really important." International and U.S.
students also tended disagree with the statement, "I could probably never
marry someone from another culture because the differences would be too much to
overcome. "
Insert Table 2 about here
Differences on items revealed that international students
were more optimistic than U.S. students that people could tolerate each other's
viewpoints. They were also more optimistic that problems can be solved through
negotiation. International students were additionally more likely than U.S.
students to believe that they could adjust to life in a wheelchair. This
contrasted, however, with the finding that U.S. students were more comfortable
than international students when seeing someone with a disability.
Attitudes toward homosexuality.
International and U.S. students also tended to hold fairly similar attitudes in
this category as well (see Table 3) . Both groups tended to agree, for example,
that job discrimination against gays was wrong.
7 Students from both groups also tended to disagree with
the statement, "Homosexuals should be isolated from heterosexuals."
They also tended to agree that gays were "like everyone else, they simply
chose an alternate lifestyle."
Insert Table 3 about here
Differences between international and U.S. students
revealed a more positive attitude by international students on two of three
items. International students tended to more strongly agree that sexual
preference should not be a factor in employment; they also tended to believe
that gays do not corrupt young people. On the third item, however, U.S.
students more strongly disagreed with the statement, "Homosexuals should
not hold leadership positions" than did international students.
Discussion
Results seem to indicate that for the most part,
international students and U . S . students were similarly tolerant in their
attitudes toward diversity. When these two groups differed, international
students tended to present the more tolerant attitudes. This differences can be
seen both in items measuring cognitive components of attitudes as well as those
items measuring behaviors. In fact, international students were significantly
more likely to have interacted with people of different cultures than U.S.
students as measured on all behavior items.
There are a several possible reasons that can be given for
these findings. International students are by virtue of their status in the
position to have interactions with persons from another country, particularly
the U.S.
8
The COAT did not specify the term "country" so
it is likely that international students interpreted this to include Americans.
By being in a foreign country, then, these students are more likely to interact
with persons who are not of the same cultural background.
But cognitive items also revealed a similarly tolerant
portrait of international students. For example, they stated that they were
comfortable interacting with people from diverse backgrounds. These students
also expressed a less ethnocentric attitude than did U. S. students, and were
more optimistic that differing viewpoints could be tolerated. These findings
are in line with Manese, Leong, and Sedlacek (1985) who found that
international students expected an environment responsive to their needs as
well as high expectations about their abilities to do well within this
environment. Part of the network of these expectations would seem to entail
students' expectations about the environment's (here the university)
responsiveness to diversity as well as their own ability to adapt to a new
environment.
Thus, if international students are to survive and succeed
on U.S. campuses, it would seem critical for them to have positive attitudes
about how the environment will respond to them, i.e., their differentness, as
well as how they will be able to handle these new situations. The current study
seems to suggest that entering international students are indeed optimistic
about the university's and their own tolerance levels. It would be interesting
to discover whether these attitudes change over the course of attendance at the
university. It would also be interesting to discover if U.S. students studying
abroad share these initially tolerant expectations about their environment as
well as their comfort levels. Another question might focus on whether portraits
of international students (including U.S. student abroad) might demonstrate
some
9
important similarities among the members of this group,
such as openness to diversity, in addition to differences (for eg., reasons for
studying abroad).
Demographically, U.S. and international students
represented quite different groups. For example, the mean age differed by
several years, international students being older. Implications for the current
study may focus on the potential incomparability of these two samples. That is,
perhaps the findings resulted because of the groups' differing ages, rather
than student status. However, data were collected for both groups during
orientation, that is, at the initial entry into the university, and the samples
are representative of the overall student population. Thus, the comparisons are
still likely to be relevant since they involve entering students' perceptions,
regardless of age. What may be concluded from this finding is that
international students represent a different population than U.S. students, so
that programming will need to reflect these differences. Part of these
differences stem from differing values (Story, 1982) , which may be represented
here in the more tolerant attitudes expressed by these students.
International and U.S. students were similar on most
attitudinal items, suggesting that both groups share generally tolerant
attitudes toward each other. But as Miville, Molla, and Sedlacek (1992) noted,
these attitudes are marked by ambivalence at the interactive level, especially
for U . S . students . Thus, it becomes imperative for institutions to initiate
programming encouraging interaction; such interaction may be viewed as a
critical step in a student's development because it promotes the development of
adaptive skills in the context of celebrating cultural differences. Interaction
could also aid in an international student's ability to cope with his or her
new surroundings. Previous research (Manese, Leong, and Sedlacek, 1985) has
shown these
10
students have limited opportunity to meet other students
and make on-campus connections, a factor which may be critical in these
students' retention. For the development of individuals from both groups, then,
universities and colleges need to devise programming that will maintain and
strengthen initially tolerant attitudes of both international and U. S.
students.
11
References
Boyan, D. R. (1981). Open doors: 1980-1981.
New York: Institute of
International Education.
Brun, M., & David, K . H . (1971) . Intercultural
communication and the
adjustment of the sojourner. Psychological
Bulletin, 76, 215-230.
Carter, R. T., & Sedlacek, W. E. (1986). Needs and
characteristics of
undergraduate international
students. Counseling Center Research Report # 1-86 .
College Park, MD: University of Maryland.
Charles, H., & Stewart, M. A . (1991) . Academic
advising of international
students. Journal of Multicultural
Counseling and Development,19, 173-181.
Hansen, G . L . (1982) . Measuring prejudice against
homosexuality
(homosexism) among college
students: A new scale. Journal of Social Psychology,117, 233-236.
Manese, J. E., Leong, F. T. L.- (1985). Background,
attitudes, and needs of
undergraduate international
students. College Student Affairs Journal, 6, 19-28.
Manese, J. E., Sedlacek, W. E., & Leong, F. T . L.
(1988) . Needs and
perceptions of female and male
international undergraduate students. Journal of Multicultural
Counseling and Development, 16, 24-29.
Miville, M. L., Molla, B., & Sedlacek, W. E. (1992) .
Attitudes of tolerance
for diversity among college
students. Journal of The Freshman Year Experience, _4, 95-110.
Sedlacek, W . E . (1987) . Black students on white
campuses: Twenty years of
Research. Journal of College
Student Personnel, 28, 484-495.
12
conflict of values? Journal of
College Student Personnel, 23, 66-70.
13
Table 1: Means and Standard Deviations for Items on
Intercultural Behavior and Contact |
|||||||
|
|
Group 1a |
|
Group 2 |
|
||
Item |
|
Mean |
SD |
|
Mean |
SD |
|
1. I have a meal with a
person from another country.* |
2.05 |
0.96 |
|
2.65 |
1.06 |
|
|
2. I attend a social event
where people from different countries participate.* |
2.06 |
0.91 |
|
2.42 |
0.96 |
|
|
3. I invite people from
another country to visit me at my residence.* |
1.81 |
0.95 |
|
2.3 |
1.02 |
|
|
4. I try to get
information about events outside the U.S.* |
2.51 |
1.06 |
|
3.52 |
1.11 |
|
|
5. Traveling in other
countries helps one appreciate different cultures.* |
1.66 |
0.92 |
|
1.4 |
0.69 |
|
|
6. There is no other
culture as good as my own.* |
3.41 |
1.19 |
|
3.84 |
0.93 |
|
|
7. I am usually
comfortable interacting with people from other countries |
2.24 |
0.76 |
|
2.25 |
0.87 |
|
|
8. When I travel, I prefer
to take a guided tour with groups of people. |
3.27 |
1.05 |
|
3.16 |
1.14 |
|
|
9. I would prefer to watch
information on TV than be a part of an audience. |
3.27 |
0.93 |
|
3.3 |
1 |
|
|
10. Traveling abroad does
not usually help foster respect for other cultures because citizens of many
countries do not like foreigners |
3.71 |
0.93 |
|
3.92 |
1.01 |
|
|
11. Traveling is often
more pleasurable if I visit places by myself than if I go with a group.* |
3.13 |
1.15 |
|
2.75 |
1.2 |
|
|
12. Cultural Differences
are so overwhelming that conflict between nations is inevitable. |
3.24 |
0.95 |
|
3.01 |
1.03 |
|
|
13.When I travel, I do not
expect to get respect from people whose culture is different than my own. |
2.3 |
0.88 |
|
2.13 |
0.85 |
|
|
14. When I travel in
another country, I would prefer to live with a family from that country than
stay in a hotel room.* |
3.4 |
1.16 |
|
2.47 |
1 |
|
Note: Responses to items 1-4 were based on the following
choices 1=Never, 2=Seldom, 3=Occasionally, 4=Often, 5=Daily. Responses to items
5-14 were based on the following choices: 1=Strongly Agree, 2=Agree, 3=Neutral,
4=Disagree, 5=Strongly Disagree. AGroup 1=U.S. Students and Group 2=International
Students.
*Means were significantly different between U.S. students and
International students (p<.05).
Table 2: Means and Standard Deviations for Items of
General Tolerance |
||||||
|
|
Group 1a |
|
Group 2 |
||
Item |
|
Mean |
SD |
|
Mean |
SD |
1. Differences of opinion
can often lead to better solutions. |
|
2.18 |
0.8 |
|
2.07 |
0.8 |
2. It saddens me that
others cannot possibly understand me because I am unique. |
|
3.37 |
0.97 |
|
3.3 |
1.08 |
3. I could adjust to life
in a wheelchair.* |
|
3.86 |
1.05 |
|
3.21 |
0.9 |
4. I do not like to
discuss social issues. |
|
3.9 |
0.94 |
|
3.8 |
0.81 |
5. People from a different
generation often don't know what is really important. |
|
3.39 |
0.97 |
|
3.4 |
1.04 |
6. It embarrasses me to
see a person who is disabled try to open a door.* |
|
3.9 |
1.02 |
|
3.56 |
1.01 |
7. I'd rather convince
people to agree with my viewpoint than to be aware of theirs. |
|
3.65 |
0.93 |
|
3.84 |
0.91 |
8. I could probably never
marry someone from another culture because the differences would be too much
to overcome. |
|
3.5 |
1.1 |
|
3.49 |
1.29 |
9. I am optimistic that
people can tolerate each other's viewpoints.* |
|
2.24 |
0.88 |
|
1.61 |
0.69 |
10. It's overoptimistic to
think that the world's problems will be solved through negotiations.* |
|
3.2 |
0.98 |
|
3.57 |
1.2 |
11. I have several friends
that are quite a bit older than I am. |
|
2.11 |
0.95 |
|
1.99 |
0.96 |
12. It can be
overwhelming, even disturbing, when one considers the many differences that
exist among people. |
|
2.8 |
1.03 |
|
2.81 |
0.8 |
Table 3: Means and Standard Deviations for Items of
Attitudes Toward Homosexuality |
||||||
|
|
Group 1a |
|
Group 2 |
||
Item |
|
Mean |
SD |
|
Mean |
SD |
1. Sexual preference
should not be a factor in employment opportunity.* |
|
2.24 |
1.17 |
|
1.81 |
0.98 |
2. Homosexuals are just
like everyone else, they simply chose an alternate lifestyle. |
|
2.76 |
1.27 |
|
2.95 |
1.41 |
3. Homosexuals should be
isolated from heterosexuals. |
|
3.52 |
1.14 |
|
3.37 |
1.23 |
4. Homosexuals should not
be discriminated against because of their sexual preference. |
|
2.51 |
1.17 |
|
2.77 |
1.23 |
5. Homosexual acts should
be illegal. |
|
3.33 |
1.24 |
|
3.15 |
1.28 |
6. Homosexuals are a
danger to our young people. |
|
3.11 |
1.27 |
|
2.97 |
1.45 |
7. I would not like to
work with a homosexual. |
|
2.85 |
1.16 |
|
2.91 |
1.15 |
8. Homosexuals should not
hold high government offices. |
|
3.31 |
1.12 |
|
3.14 |
1.08 |
9. Job discrimination
against homosexuals is wrong. |
|
2.45 |
1.1 |
|
2.54 |
0.96 |
10. Homosexuals should not
hold leadership positions.* |
|
3.48 |
1 |
|
3.1 |
1.19 |
11. Homosexuals do not
corrupt young people.* |
|
2.82 |
1.05 |
|
3.1 |
1.1 |
12. I would not want a
homosexual to live in the house (apartment) next to mine. |
|
2.76 |
1.15 |
|
2.81 |
1.21 |
13. If I found out my
friend was a homosexual, our friendship would be damaged. |
|
3.1 |
1.15 |
|
3.15 |
1.86 |
14. I would never have
anything to do with a person if I knew he/she was a homosexual. |
|
3.5 |
1.09 |
|
3.23 |
1.13 |
15. Apartment complexes
should not accept homosexuals as renters. |
|
3.68 |
1.06 |
|
3.6 |
1.1 |