COUNSELING
CENTER
UNIVERSITY
OF MARYLAND
COLLEGE
PARK, MARYLAND 20742
Forty
Years of Using Labels to Communicate About Nontraditional Students: Does It
Help or Hurt?
Franklin
D. Westbrook and William E. Sedlacek
Research
Report #9-91
COUNSELING
CENTER
UNIVERSITY
OF MARYLAND
COLLEGE
PARK, MARYLAND 20742
Franklin
D. Westbrook and William E. Sedlacek
Research
Report #9-91
Summary
Forty Years of Using Labels to Communicate About Nontraditional Students: Does It Help or Hurt?
This manuscript deals with two
topics: How the education literature describes minority students, and how to
effectively communicate with minority students. An analysis of articles in the
Education Index since 1950 showed that despite increased concern for minorities
in the literature, the labels used to describe them may have done much to
exacerbate problems. The oldest term expressing concern for minorities was
"acculturation," and "intercultural education" was the most
popular in the 1950's. In the 1960's "culturally deprived" was the
most common. References to cultural deprivation remained common in the 1970's
but "multicultural education" became popular in the late 1970's and
through the 1980's.
When talking to minority
students the writers recommend using Bowen's triangulation concept to discuss
racism and negotiating the system. Handling racism has been shown to be a
critical ability in minority student retention by Tracey and Sedlacek. Bowen
suggests that one learns ways to negotiate the racist system from parents and
grandparents; the individual being the third point in the triangle. Under
stress a person tends to regress and rely more on learning from previous
generations.
It is not clear what it will
take to eliminate the distance that has developed between U.S. ethnic groups,
but it is clear that only communications unfettered by suspicion will enable fair-minded
individuals to work together to discover solutions.
Forty Years of Using Labels to Communicate About Nontraditional Students: Does It Help or Hurt?
Over the past 20 years African
American students have matriculated in increasingly greater numbers at
primarily White educational institutions. There has been less legal segregation
against Native Americans, Hispanics, and Asian Americans, than that which
restricted African Americans. However, these nontraditional groups have begun
to attend primarily White educational institutions in greater numbers. This
influx of nontraditional students has changed both the demographic character
and the philosophy of these institutions in most profound ways.
United States colleges and
universities have for generations welcomed foreign students. Until 1964, the
students were primarily White and had plans of either going back home where
they would become ambassadors of goodwill for the U.S.A., or of remaining in
the U.S.A. to integrate relatively freely and easily into the White population.
The culture of the student body was not in question. Since 1964 much of this
has changed. The foreign born and the nontraditional student populations who
represent all of the racial groups mentioned above, enrolled in the higher
education in greater numbers. Many had decided they could neither integrate nor
assimilate, and many did not have other nations and homes to which to return.
Vocal members of ethnic groups across the country made demands that their
rights be recognized.
Glazier and Moynihan (1963) had noted,
"The notion that the intense and unprecedented mixture of ethnic and
religious groups in American life was soon to blend into a homogeneous end
product has outlived its usefulness." The institutions needed to learn to
accommodate the newly arrived, nontraditional student without expecting to
change anything about them other than their information base. One way they
sought to do this was by developing educational, social, and financial support
programs within the institutions (Pate and Garcia, 1981).
The philosophy of higher education
was constructed along pragmatic lines. It tolerated a few voices, in the name
of free speech and academic freedom, that criticized the separate but equal
laws, but it cooperated with segregation and resisted change along with other
elements of the society. The Civil Rights Laws of 1964 exposed the university
to native born, nontraditional students who expected an education but did not
necessarily expect to acculturate. All of them planned to stay here, and many
of them planned to retain their differences (Vela, 1977 and Henkin, 1985).
Because African American
students had been so completely cut off from the opportunity structure, they
often came to higher education behind White students in college preparation
(Scanzoni, 1970). Their lack of preparation was understandable from the
perspective of expectation theory. They came to college because the opportunity
presented itself; they came less than adequately prepared because they had not
expected to have the opportunity to come (Rosenthal and Jacobson, 1968), but
they came with many of the same values, aspirations, and traditions as those
held by the White population.
To a much greater extent,
however, Native Americans, Hispanics, and Asian Americans came with national
identities, which they, as groups, intended to maintain while asserting their
rights as United States citizens. This posed an identity problem for the
institutions. The institutions had needs to assert their level of compliance
with laws that mandate the enrollment of minority students; and they had needs
to demonstrate the extent to which they attempted to serve special needs for
the different groups; but they also had needs to demonstrate that their images
remained in tact.
In terms of dealing with the
nontraditional student, two problems presented themselves: First, how
institutional representatives should talk about the issues that related to
nontraditional students; and second, how they should talk to nontraditional
students with whose groups they had only stereotyped familiarity? These were
large issues for higher education personnel because from the recruitment
process through the service practices that must be used for the students'
benefit, communications is the basic tool.
Some of the larger problems have
come during efforts to communicate about African American students. Service
personnel have had to be more concerned about those African American students
who are inadequately prepared for the institutions'
3
academic requirements than about those who are well prepared.
Thus, in discussing minority students, they have found themselves "falling
into the trap of identification and diagnosis by terminology rather than
etiology" (Lipton, 1962). They have referred to the students as
disadvantaged, culturally disadvantaged, educationally disadvantaged,
economically disadvantaged, socially disadvantaged, culturally deprived,
educationally deprived, economically deprived, socially deprived, etc.
(Bossone, 1970; Glatt, 1965; Congreve, 1966; Hawk, 1965, 1967; Henson, 1973; Weiner
and Murray, 1963).
Deprivation is a major issue for
the minority populations that have been so affected. They have a history of
having been deprived of freedom, of human rights, of equal education, and of
dignified jobs. Declaring them to be culturally deprived leads the White
majority into scapegoating the persons who have the problems that attend
deprived backgrounds, and away from their responsibilities as educational,
political, and psychological practitioners (Buchheimer, 1968).
Benelli, Arcuri, and Marchesini
(1988) indicated that adults adjust their definitional criteria to meet the
needs of their audience. If this be the case, the use of cultural deprivation
to define characteristics of ethnic groups who show differences from those of
the definer's group, means they and the audience judge the described peoples'
background in terms of good versus bad, important versus unimportant, and
usefulness versus uselessness (Lipton, 1962).
"In other words," said
Lipton (1962, p.8), "our definition reflects our cultural backgrounds and
attitudes which are indicative of the middle class concept of culture which may
involve knowledge, attitudes, learning, and understandings in . . . music, art,
academic apperceptions, social relationships, emotional development, and a
whole gamut of intangible elements which transcend racial, religious and socio-economic
lines." He went on to say that the use of the word "deprivation"
suggests a feeling of superiority and "other patterns of non-accepting
attitudes" which are basically rejecting. Such attitudes appear negative,
aggressive, and rejecting and contribute to, but was not totally responsible
for, the problems they encounter as new arrivals on campus.
Many Native Americans assert
their cultural identities through the maintenance of ties to their various
national identities. Many Hispanics and Asian Americans assert their cultural
identities through the maintenance of their language and other traditions. The
students who come from these families, to a significant extent, retain those
cultural identities. African Americans have a rich tradition in America, but
they have neither another language, nor a collected memory of themselves as
inhabitants of other lands. Their ample contributions to, and involvement with,
the national life have been noted by Harper (1973) and Griffith (1978), and
they appear to represent an investment in the national culture.
5
Culture is not neatly defined in
the minds of social scientists. Rather, it is a human structure which has
variations in meaning depending upon the branch of social science from which
the definition is provided. Although "no one person has internalized it
all," the explicit portions of a culture are "internalized by
participant individuals. A composite (of a culture) exists in the total group
as well as being manifested in its artifacts" (Kluckhohn,1962, p.72).
"Culture consists of
patterns, explicit and implicit, of and for behavior acquired and transmitted
by symbols, constituting the distinctive achievement of human groups, including
their embodiments in artifacts; the essential core of culture consists of
traditional (i.e., historically derived and selected) ideas and especially
their attached values; culture systems may, on the one hand, be considered as
products of action, on the other as conditioning influences upon further
action" (Kluckhohn, 1962, p. 73) .
By the Kluckhohn definition of
culture, African Americans' contributions in the areas of art, music,
literature, and technology are etched so deeply into the fabric of the United
States that they cannot be extracted. In spite of those who may dislike the
fact, African Americans appear to be as much a part of the "American
culture" as any of those persons who claim it (Franklin, 1967 and Woodson
and Wesley, 1966).
But higher education personnel
lump all of the nontraditional students together as culturally different. The current
strategies for communicating with them are called "cross-cultural"
Because nontraditional students are clearly from different ethnic groups than
the White ethnic groups represented in the traditional student body, college
and university personnel think they have to use different strategies than the
ones they use when they attempt to help White students. Although this may be
true, it does not necessarily follow that nontraditional students come from a
different culture, and such a generalization should never be made. Some of the
issues have to do with eye contact, proximity during a conversation,
directiveness versus the providing of options, and self disclosure (Keneshige,
1973; Sue & Sue, 1973; Watanabe, 1973; Littlefield, 1974; Ruiz &
Padilla, 1977; and Seligman, 1977).
The problem with attempting to
use special strategies that are designated to satisfy nontraditional students'
needs in E:;, of these areas is that the needs do not generalize well either
within or across groups. Some students react differently in interpersonal
situations than would many other individuals from their group. Higher education
personnel need to be sensitive to the interactional needs of groups of
nontraditional students, but more than that, they need a set of strategies that
will enable them to establish rapport with any individuals that arepotentially
different from themselves in interpersonal interaction styles.
The purposes of this study were
to survey educational and psychological literature, as cataloged in the
Education Index,
7
for (1) labels used in talking about nontraditional
students and (2) understandings about problems in inter-ethnic
interactions, or problems in talking to (i.e. developing and maintaining
rapport with) nontraditional students.
TALKING
ABOUT NONTRADITIONAL STUDENTS
Procedures
In an effort to explore appropriate ways of referring to nontraditional students, the last 40 years of educational and psychological articles, as indexed by the Education Index, were surveyed. In the Education Index subject headings are written to reflect the terminology used in articles. The headings change as new terminology dictates. When new terminology begins to appear, the user is alerted to this phenomenon by "See references" from the old terminology to the new.
In the next section will be
found one table which lists terminology that is used to identify articles that
were published about minority people and related issues, and one table which
shows how cross-cultural is typically used in educational and
psychological literature. The key to entering the Education Index was the term
"disadvantaged." Articles that were published under a given label
were counted. The point at which the term branched or was retired was noted by
a new entry. The new terms were added to the table, and the same process was
repeated. The survey began with indexed articles on January 1, 1950 and ended with
articles indexed through December 31, 1989.
8
Results and Discussion
1950-1959 - Focus on Acculturation
In early 1950's there was
relatively little concern expressed in the educational literature about the
second-class citizenship that had been imposed on minority groups in the
country. The oldest term found in the literature that reflected concern about
them was "acculturation."
"Acculturation is a process
of developing one culture system out of two or more culture systems whose human
representatives are in contact with each other . . . . It occurs to a degree in
the realm of the unconscious and a new culture mosaic becomes accepted before
its subjects are aware of its adoption" (Bogardus 1950, p. 203). The process
through which acculturation takes place can be (a) blind, (b) imposed, or (c)
democratic. Democratic acculturation is thought to be the best of the types It
starts with the assumption that all cultures are equal and served the purposes
of the groups who caused them to evolve. However, culture, is perfect and
herein lies the advantage of having people from other cultures immigrate. They
can bring culture patterns that can be useful to the host culture (Bogardus,
1950).
When acculturation evolves
imperfectly, friction can develop among the different ethnic groups.
Intercultural education i6 process which has been developed to study such
conflicts, and to provide resolution for them (Bogardus, 1949). An
intercultural workshop was developed in the Bureau of Intercultural Education
9
at the University of Southern California, and it was
devoted largely to moderating tensions that developed in the Los Angeles public
school system among ethnic, racial, and religious groups (Cole, 1946).
The indexing of an average of 185 journals over the period
of 1950 through 1959 showed that an average of about two articles per year were
published on the topic of acculturation (See Table 1, Column 1). Writers,
however, were generally more concerned about foreign nationals being
acculturated.
Acculturation had been in use for some years and had
branched into "intercultural" concerns. There were a number of
articles indexed under intercultural education (M=15.6 per year) which
discussed various racial and religious concerns, but was largely international
in focus, including education concerning learning more about the cultures of
individuals who migrated to the United States.
There was, however, a radical shift in authors' interest
in issues indexed as "racially different." Interest in racially
different issues went from an average of roughly three per year in 1950-1957
to an average of 10 per year in 1958-1959. This was the period during
which African Americans, under the leadership of the National Association for
the Advancement of Colored People, the Southern Christian Leadership
Conference, the Congress of Racial Equality, and the Student Nonviolent
Coordinating Committee, made a vigorous push for human rights, public
accommodations, and equal education in general. Under the
10
banner of "scientific" education and psychology,
writers were raising issues which would ultimately be used to delay African
American's enjoyment of opportunities in the United States.
1960-1969 - Focus on Differences, Deprivation
and Disadvantagement
During the period 1960 through 1969 an average of 291
articles which relate to minority issues were indexed (See Table 1, Column 2).
Interests in acculturation remained at about the same level while interests in
intercultural education dropped a bit. Acculturation branched a second time
into "culturally different" which was a subtle but aggressive and
regressive shift. It was directed away from the consideration of cultural
groups being equals (Bogardus, 1949) to a focus on differences.
Some examples of how
writers were thinking about non-Anglo ethnic groups are as follows:
African American parents and their children differed from middle class--primarily
White groups—in their willingness to persevere to goals in the face of
obstacles, and they differed in their assessment of real versus ideal goals (Weiner
and Murray, 1963).
"The advantaged person, on the whole, takes pride in
himself and his life. He feels comfortable in, and able to cope with, our
complex society. His values are in order. He knows the difference between right
and wrong. He has the social skills get along effectively with his fellowman,
and he accepts some responsibility toward those less fortunate than himself. He
has
11
a purpose in life. He is aware of his abilities, and he is
reasonably certain he can achieve his goals.
"The disadvantaged person has difficulty thinking
well of himself, and he uses devious ways to justify his existence. He feels he
has been cheated of the chance to develop into a successful person. Fair play
and moral and social responsibility make little sense to him. He is reticent or
insecure about his abilities and unsure of his goals. Often the disadvantaged
person is convinced that he cannot achieve any commendable goals"
(Congreve, 1966, p. 4).
"Disadvantaged children are usually too demoralized
and frustrated, and too powerless to combat the forces that confuse and ensnare
their lives. They 'know' they are failures, and they are convinced they always
will be. They live in defeat and despair, and feel inferior and exiled from the
prevailing society. The majority are too disillusioned and dispirited to care.
They have been rejected and discouraged too many times to have any ideas of
hope or ambition. They will not even try to do what is necessary to escape
their deprivation -- stay in school for instance.
"Also, the disadvantaged cannot cope with
humiliation, nor can they assimilate an attack on their dignity or values.
Resentment, intense anxiety, and often direct hostility are manifested among
these adolescents. Any aspect of authority: their parents, their teachers, the
law, the school, is a direct target for their anger. Similarly, emotional
disorders requiring
12
specialized treatment are common among many of these
children" (Ornstein, 1966, p. 155).
The last example establishes an interesting equation
between "deprivation" and "differences." Glatt (1965) said,
"deprivation can quite obviously be related to certain ethnic and
religious groups, social classes. and residential districts. But to say so is
to invite a whirlwind of unfavorable reaction," and for that reason
authors are less clear about cultural deprivation than they could be. He goes
on to be clear by saying, "Common and often easily identified causes of
deprivation are economic status, housing, previous formal education, physical
and mental health, and access to religious instruction. Other just as
significant but less readily specified reasons for differences include family
attitudes and values, use of free time, maturation, interests, appreciations,
and accessibility of learning materials outside of school.
"The students coming into our schools bring with them
diverse backgrounds and reflect differences based on these and. other causes.
Growth and development that occur in the classroom often tend to accentuate
rather than to ameliorate such differences" (Glatt, 1965, p. 6).
This is the period during which the first significant
Civil Rights legislation, since the Supreme Court's 1954 decision declaring
segregated schools unconstitutional, was passed. "Culturally
different," which later elaborated into "cultural differences,"
reflected authors' recognition of the ground swell
13
of concern in the educational community for how to deal
with unfamiliar ethnic groups who were rapidly becoming the majority in urban
schools. The "cultural differences" literature often purported to
specify what the differences were and that they could often be found in the
communities represented by Native Americans, Hispanics, and African Americans
who were "culturally deprived" (Riga, 1977), and proclaimed their
needs for specially trained teachers.
"Racially different" gave way to "race
differences" which grew steadily from its entry into the Index in 1961
through the end of the decade. "Race discrimination" and "race
prejudice" became higher level concerns and reached their peak during the
period 1964 through 1969. Many authors were interested in understanding the
reasons behind the inner city riots, which were labeled the "long hot
summers."
"Racism" arose as one of the answers (Report of
the National Advisory Commission on Civil Disorders, 1968), however, many
articles were critical of the African Americans who reacted with riotous
behavior to historical frustrations as well as to events such as the murder of
Dr. Martin Luther King, Jr. Concerns for racial tolerance and "race
relations" were raised to their highest levels during this period.
1970-1979 - Focus on Culture Specific
Differences
During the period 1970 through 1979 authors' interests in
cultural deprivation and the culture-specific labels arose. Under the 11
culture-specific labels beginning with "cross
14
cultural studies" and ending with "education for
the culturally deprived children" (See Table 1, Column 3) more than 1000
articles were published. Henson (1973) took the position that writers and
speakers on cultural deprivation or the general topic of "disadvantagement"
had not been careful to talk about the same things. Their messages and
presentations were therefore inconsistent and basically negative. He discussed
an instance in which one writer, David Coyle, had labeled as
"unlucky" "American Indians, Mexican-Americans, Negroes, migrant
Americans, and adolescents." At a later date, an educator cited Coyle's
"unlucky" group along with others and labeled all of them
"disadvantaged." Henson thought many groups of Americans had a way of
life which left them somehow short of the ability to achieve the education that
is currently needed. He thought that "Programs and people are needed which
will focus on helping identify these special needs and to find ways to filling
them" (p. 120) but without confusing the issue with inconsistent labels.
Hawk (1967) believed the
"disadvantaged" students' problem to be one of a negative self-concept.
He cited research to support his notion that their problems are resolvable (1)
if they are given experiences which encourage them to think of themselves as equal
and acceptable to people around them, (2) to think of themselves as the prime
determiners of their behavior, and (3) to view themselves as adequate,
valuable, and worthy but not through the environment of the integrated school
(busing was at that time
15
a primary mode of accomplishing integrated schools). While
several studies had shown that upward social mobility is more probable when the
lower-class child has access to the behavior patterns and the value
standards of the middle-class child, Hawk (1967) found that
"culturally deprived" children in "predominantly deprived
schools received more friendship choices and leadership choices from their
classmates," which suggests an environment which can support the building
and maintenance of a positive self concept.
Bossone (1970) discussed the
problems of teachers and administrators who were motivated to do an effective
job of educating ethnic group children but who were defeated by their inept
educational backgrounds. He said "Education must place more emphasis upon
affective and less upon cognitive objectives; that is, there must be more
attention given to the concerns of the learner and not just the concerns of the
academic discipline."
While each of the latter
positions appears to attempt to take some of the negative valence away from the
massive aversive characterization of nontraditional students, they raise their
own "yellow flags." Nontraditional students are certain they have not
enjoyed sufficiently concentrated concern. Henson (1973) would dilute what they
get. Hawk (1967) made a credible case for the self-concept issue, but his
solution cut across the educational environment strategies that had long been
accepted as important to the achievement of equal education. Bossone (1970)
16
made appropriate criticisms of professionals' failing to
teach children of ethnic group families, but he appeared to focus away from the
necessity of the achievement of cognitive objectives, which was precisely the
problem.
1980-1989 - Focus on Multicultural Education
During the period 1980's acculturation continued at a low
level of interest to authors, and intercultural education and intergroup
education elaborated into assimilation, or merging into the dominant culture,
and multicultural education (See Table 1, Column 4). The label of multicultural
education (M=44.7) suggested authors' interest in recognizing the existence of
other ethnic groups, but the impulse had inherent problems. It assumed an
"ours versus theirs" view toward culture. It also risked confusing
ethnic with cultural issues, and in the process paying inappropriate levels of
attention to them, e.g., rating cultures based on the nutritional contents of
certain preferred foods when it is probably an ethnic issue.
The use of culture to characterize interactions among U.S
ethnic groups is questionable. When people from different cultures live side by
side, they tend to adopt the parts of each others culture that they feel to be
superior to their own (Bogardus, 1949). Although it is possible for groups to
clash because of conflicting traditions, problems between people are likely to
be at a less macro-level.
Pate and Garcia (1981) attempted to study the extent of
multi-ethnic/multi-cultural programs in schools represented by
17
700 members of a national social studies supervisors
organization. They received a 21 percent return rate from a questionnaire,
which was too low for generalization, other than perhaps, in a negative way.
Many of the programs appeared to have been put together in a thoughtless
fashion, few had any standard form of evaluation, few had human relations
goals, and some of the programs appeared to have been traditional courses in
traditional academic divisions rather than special ways of treating content
about other cultures.
Ornstein and Levine (1982) noted that, "multicultural
education can be indispensable in helping to achieve constructive cultural
pluralism in a nation composed of diverse ethnic groups. At the same time,
however, multicultural education can be potentially harmful or damaging. In
general, the potential dangers of multicultural education are the same as those
associated with the larger concept of cultural pluralism. The major dangers
according to Ornstin and Levine are:
(1) "Multicultural education can emphasize separatism
in a way that is divisive and disunifying:"
a)Emphasis on differences may
give impetus to those who always wanted and continue to want segregation.
b)Emphasis on differences may
make it impossible to develop a citizenry which values a national identity.
(2) "Multicultural education may be used to justify
second class
education;"
a)Programs
may be generally thought to be second rate.
18
b) The Pate and Garcia
experiences above may be cases in
point.
(3) "Multicultural education may lead to
fragmentation of the
school curriculum;"
a)Irrelevant or at best
elementary facts about an ethnic group may be substituted for more relevant information.
b)Simplistic presentations of
material about ethnic groups, when it could otherwise be integrated into general
information of the topic, could be more damaging than helpful to the group.
Ornstein and Levine further reflected upon the general reversal in the Nation
in the early 1970's on the notion of the "melting pot" in favor of
cultural pluralism. What they failed to mention is the fact that the reversal
coincides (a) with the periods when non-White ethnic groups, African
Americans in particular, were demanding equal treatment and (b) with the period
when the culturally deprived labels became so prominent in the educational
literature.
Cross cultural studies (M=31.1) is another way of saying
"we are different." It pays attention to differences rather than to
similarities, and the differences observed were of people who, by the
observers' definition, could not cope. Authors had become so convinced that
culturally deprived was an appropriate descriptive label for Hispanic, Native
American, and African American students (Higa, 1977), although primarily the
latter, that they had begun to devote their time to prescribing the kinds of
services that were needed by these groups. There were high
19
priorities placed on counseling services (M=17.6) and
education for counselors (M=53.6) and, to a lesser extent, for education for
psychologists, teachers in training, inservice teachers, and for culturally
deprived children.
Of the hundreds of articles
that were written about different aspects of "cultural deprivation,"
few statements in any of those that were reviewed could be read by ethnic
minority persons without some loss of pride. But another group of articles
categorized as cross cultural studies were of a very different character.
Over the past few years, cross cultural has become more often
used to describe interactions between ethnic minority individuals and White
persons. It should be noted, however, that this trend, regarding inter-ethnic
interactions, has not been picked up in the indexed literature where it has
been in use otherwise for over 20 years.
As can be noted from Table 2, since 1970 over 550 articles
have been indexed under "cross cultural studies." Over 60 percent of
these articles have the name of a foreign country, e.g., Sweden, as one source
of data for the study. In the remaining 30+ percent articles, there are also
likely some with a foreign country data source. This means that over thelife of
the use of the term, cross cultural has been used primarily to signify
interactions between persons from the United States and some other country.
20
If non-White ethnic minority groups cooperate in
having themselves labeled with any of the current terms that include
"cultural," they have a choice between being put-down with
"culturally deprived," or of being put-out (of the national
citizenship network) with "cross cultural." On the other hand,
"inter-ethnic" logically describes interactions between or
among ethnic groups, of which the United States has many. Several are White;
several are Hispanic; several are African American; several are Native
American; and several are Asian American. Ethnic groups are assumed to have
human rights within the Nation. The constitution does not permit de jure
discrimination. If labels are essential to communications, the best that can be
done is to begin with one that treats all groups equally. Interethnic appears
to have the edge on any label that has "cultural" in its name.
There were still strong needs to discuss race differences
(M=39.9), and there was little concern that this contributed to racial
discrimination or prejudice. There was, however, heightened concern that racism
was somehow involved in the problems between the races, and expressions of
needs for race relations (M=18.9) activities were at the second highest level
for the 40 year period.
Conclusions
Over the 40 year period during which articles were
reviewed, the professions moved from relatively little concern for the plight
of African Americans who were denied human rights, to a
21
great deal of attention which might at times be mistaken
for concern. As a matter of fact, many of the expressions probably did more to
exacerbate problems for the groups than they did to help.
Buchheimer (1968) and others saw the
"disadvantaged" label as scapegoating persons to whom opportunities
had been denied. It probably affected the self esteem of some individuals
(Brookover, Erickson, and Joiner, 1967; and Epps, 1969), discouraged others,
contributed to the suspicion of still others, and got the latter labeled by
what Grier and Cobb (1968) and Ridley (1984) called "healthy cultural
paranoia."
Sedlacek and Brooks (1970), in a national survey of
university admissions personnel, reported that authors recognized that Black
and disadvantaged were not synonymous. They also discussed Blacks having had
different cultural experiences without attributing "cultural
differences" to the individuals. Kluckhohn's (1962) definition of culture
raises profound questions about the appropriateness of labeling U.S. minorities
with any kind of cultural term because of the experiences they have had in the
country.
Clearly all of the worlds' racial types are represented
among the minority groups under discussion. Sedlacek and Brooks (1976)
advocated acknowledging those differences, but the context is important. When
it is done in the context of a workshop to reduce racism, as they suggested,
the purposes for doingthe exercise can be discussed and participants can adjust
erroneous
22
impressions given or mistaken impressions received. When
individuals take it upon themselves to counsel others in writing, the best
intended communication can be mis-stated or mis-read with little
chance of amends ever being made.
Communicating about minority groups can be a hazardous or
cumbersome exercise. Calling them disadvantaged, deprived, or by any other
deficiency label can be disastrous for communications. The differences among
the groups in question are probably ethnic and racial, and nothing else can be
certain until a number of hypotheses have been tested with each individual
encountered. Since "communicating about" suggests the absence of the
objects of the communication, no hypothesis testing is possible. If
identification of the group interaction is essential to the communication,
inter-ethnic would appear to be appropriately descriptive and sufficient.
Counseling between two individuals from groups that have a
history of different customs or characteristics, or a different language or
common history is by definition inter-ethnic counseling. There is no
reason, however, to elevate such interactions to the level of cross or inter-cultural
interaction. A culture, according to Kluckhohn, can accommodate many different
ethnic groups. To be an integral part of the culture, they need only contribute
to the patterns of behaviors ideas, values, and the transmittal of the same to
successive generations. Griffith (1978) suggested that such contributions
23
by African Americans are well known in the history of the
United States.
It is not respectful of a group, as some would declare, to
infer a cultural difference between two individuals simply because they are
from different racial or ethnic heritages, or because one has more money than
the other. To do so, in the United States of America, is to infer a superiority-inferiority
continuum between the two "ethnic" representatives. It is unlikely,
under current circumstances, that ethnic minority groups in the United States
could feel complimented by such a characterization.
This area raises issues which are relevant to
professionals who work with individuals on one-to-one, small group,
and small gathering (as in workshops) bases, and possibly not for others.
"Talking to" here assumes that the speaker is in a situation in which
there is a two-way communication. Westwood and Ishiyama (1990) proposed
that the communication process is an intervention for client change, in and of
itself, and not just as a medium through which change concepts are implemented.
This makes it imperative that the communication carry with it valence that
supports the general purposes of the speaker.
Although some of Griffith's (1978) conclusions are
arguable, he made a credible case regarding how the primarily White university
environment impresses African American students, as opposed to how it impresses
the White population for whom it was
24
designed. One of the reasons is that for many African
Americans, being at the university is the first time they have been in such close
contact with White persons who are obviously in control of
them. While White students are also controlled, they may
have had more prior experience being in such situations.
Potkay and Fullerton (1973), from a primarily White sample
of college students, found complaints among them which they labeled
"negotiating the university system." In an independent study,
Westbrook and Smith (1976) found that African American students were particularly
impressed by the control that primarily White university personnel had over
them. Some examples are, "who they had to talk to in order to resolve
particular problems," "how to determine who would make decisions
them," etc. These problems were characterized the same as Potkay and
Fullerton had, i.e. "negotiating the university system." The basic
issue in here is the effect of talking to university personnel.
Westbrook, Miyares, and Roberts (1978) administered a 20
item Student Problem Survey to samples of African American students at an
African American university, and African American and White students at a White
university. There were several interesting items that were significantly
different between African American students and White students, but negotiating
university system was not one of them.
Negotiating the university system is of interest here for
two reasons: (1) In the Westbrook, et al (1978) study both
25
African American and White students felt more intense
about the effects of talking to university personnel (negotiating the
university system) than they did about some of the items where there were
significant differences between them, but their levels of intensity were
relatively equal; and (2) Tracey and Sedlacek (1987) have a scale on their Non-cognitive
Questionnaire, "understanding and dealing with racism," which
measures very similar concerns.
One level of assessment on the Tracey and Sedlacek racism
scale is directed at determining the extent to which the person understands
racism. The second level is an attempt to determine the extent to which the
person is willing to confront racism in a healthy manner. Individuals who are
in vulnerable positions are encouraged to confront racism when there can be
important positive outcomes, but not when negative outcomes are more likely
than positive ones.
Family systems theory provides a model for beginning a
positive communication process with nontraditional students. Radke-Yarrow,
Trager, and Miller (1952) found some verification for parents exercising a
major function of teaching children how to relate to the outside world. Bowen
(1978) proposed that this instruction, either direct or indirect, goes back as
far as three generations through a process of projection. How independently
persons interact with the outside world is determined by the persons' level of
differentiation from her/his family.
26
If persons remain undifferentiated from their family of
orientation, they interact through the process of triangulation. Triangulation
describes the way three people relate to each other around emotional issues
that are among them. A triangle is the smallest unit of any stable relationship
system. Dyads function efficiently until anxiety arises, at which time the
third party in the relationship triangle is used to bring stability to the
dyadic relationship. Triangles develop in families through the "family
projection process." Bowen (1978) indicated that the triangulation process
begins before the birth of a child as the mother (parents) plan(s) for its
coming (p. 127). After the child is born, the parents begin to project
expectations upon him/her.
The child typically accepts the projections and may in addition
introject some of the parental values on its own accord The child may
incorporate parental values, attitudes, and characteristics that range from
minor and inconsequential to major and pronounced. It is through this process
that the individual remains involved to some extent with past generations.
Triangles are normal
and natural building blocks for relationships units. They exist in all
families, but few if any family members are carbon copies of their nuclear
family systems. The variable which makes this possible is called
differentiation. The extent to which successive generations differ from their
ancestors is a function of individual differentiation and
the merging of alien members into new nuclear family units.
27
Several researchers have commented upon the differences
they have observed between the ways some Asian American (Sue and Sue, 1973),
Native American (Henkin, 1985), Hispanic (Vela, 1977), and African American and
White (Ridley, 1984) students conduct themselves when they are interviewed by
an "authority figure."
Radke, Trager, and Davis (1949) studied the social
perceptions and attitudes about racial matters in African American and White
children from kindergarten through second grade in a northeastern city. The
researchers showed the children a group of pictures and asked them questions
about the pictorial content. In one of the pictures, an African American child
was standing off alone while a group of White children played. The following
are excerpts of typical responses by grade when the interviewers showed the
picture and asked, "Will they (the other children) ask him to play? They
all answered no and added: White child, second grade--"Because
he is colored and don't want to play with white boys, and white people don't
want to play with him because they know he cheats and is too tough."
Second child, second grade--"All of them go away and leave
this little boy alone. That is what happens around my street." White
child, kindergarten--"A colored boy is in our neighborhood and
we don't play with
28
him. I don't trust him A colored pulled my hair in this
school, so I ain't playing with
no more niggers." White child, kindergarten--"I
know (the little boy is
colored). We have colored people in our neighborhood. My
father wants to get out of
that neighborhood because he doesn't like colored
people." Negro child, second grade-
"They won't let him play. They don't like colored.
"Negro child, first grade--"Colored boy can't play with
whites" (p. 367).
The researchers reported that most of the children showed
some reluctance to talk about racial issues. When they were prodded to give
their impressions of the pictures, the White children quickly lost their
resistance to talking. On the other hand, a significant number of African
American children showed their inhibitions to talking about race by never
mentioning it without being asked. Another percentage of them showed discomfort
and anxiety as they cooperated.
Radke-Yarrow,
Trager, and Miller (1952) replicated the study with parents and children and
found identical attitudes among the children, and they found that the parents
either taught negative ideas to their children, discussed racial matters in
earshot of the children, or restricted the children's activities such that they
got the ideas they expressed at school. The researchers
29
concluded that the parents in both groups communicated
prejudices and misconceptions to their children and gave explanations that
would neither represent true racial differences nor would they solve racial
problems.
Ridley (1984) discussed, the
disinclination of African American students to self disclose to a White
counselor. He quoted Grier and Cobb (1968) who stated (p. 149) that "it is
necessary for a black man in America to develop a profound distrust of his
white fellow citizens . . . If he does not so protect himself, he will live a
life of such pain and shock as to find life itself unbearable." Myrdal (1944)
reported that because of their experiences in a segregated society where they
had only the most primitive human rights, African Americans were reluctant to
talk to White people about anything. Grier and Cobb and Ridley called this
defensive behavior "normal cultural paranoia." Ashby (1986),
Bronstein (1986), and Thompson, Neville, Weathers, Poston, and Atkinson (1990)
take issue with the "paranoia" part of the description. Thompson et
al, renamed the response "racism reaction." None of the authors was
willing to "blame the victims" of segregation, discrimination, and
rejection for acting cautiously in the presence of a White person. \
An Example Using Bowen's Differentiation Concept
Westbrook and Sedlacek (1988)
used the Bowen differentiation concept in a series of vignettes to teach
workshop participants how to begin to obviate the racism reaction (Thompson, et
al.
30
1990) as a barrier to their communications with ethnic
minority students. One of the vignettes has to do with taking a hypothetical
student of 18 years old and assigning an average age, at the student's birth,
to his/her same-sex parent, e.g., 25. The 18-year-old student's parent
would then be 43. One then assigns a similar child-birth age to the
student's grandparent and great-grandparent.
The hypothetical student's age is 18 and was born in 1973,
the parent is 43 and was born in 1948, the grandparent is 68 and was born in
1923, and the great-grandparent is 93 and was born in 1898. Participants
were then asked to remember what was happening in the United States for African
Americans, Asian Americans, Hispanics, and Native Americans at times that
parallel the different generations for the student's family. Within these time
lines major periods of official disadvantagement were either in process or
recently past. Of course, workers with ethnic minority students need to know
this history (Griffith, 1978; and Westwood and Ishiyama, 1990).
Reasoning from Bowen's position, it must be assumed that
ethnic minority students know things, which they learned from their families,
which justify the racism reaction. Whether or not their suspicions about a
given majority worker are true, the majority workers who want to communicate
effectively with them need to be willing to do what it takes to build rapport
with the helpee. They will need either to reflect upon, research, or
31
speculate about the messages they themselves got in their
families about ethnic minority people, and how their attitudes may affect their
working relationships. They will need to understand their level of
differentiation from detrimental attitudes and values that might have existed
in their families, and be willing to discuss these matters with the student in
order to clear the air.
Communicating effectively with minority students may be no
more difficult than it is to admit that one may be among those persons in our
culture who have had, although unsolicited, advantages that have given them a
head start on certain ethnic groups in the country. An act of this type may
serve to remove barriers caused by the racism reaction, which Schmedinghoff
(1977) believed to exist.
There has been a great deal of
scapegoating of ethnic groups in our culture. The labeling has often prevented
students at all levels to prepare to contribute optimally to the culture. The
assigning of deficiency labels neither relieves educators of the responsibility
of educating students nor does it deny students learning opportunities.
Students learn outside of school, and they may learn things that are most
beneficial for the culture.
The Report of the National
Advisory Commission on civil Disorders (1968) said "Our Nation is moving
toward two societies, one white and one black -- separate and
unequal" (p. 1). We are 23 years closer to that point. It is not clear
what it will take
to eliminate the distance that has developed between
ethnic
32
groups, but it is clear that only communications
unfettered by suspicion will enable fair minded individuals to work together to
discover solutions. Negative labels will not help.
33
References
Ashby, H. (1986). Mislabeling the black client: A reply to
Ridley. American Psychologist, 41, 224-225.
Benelli, B., Arcuri, L., & Marchesini, G. (1988).
Cognitive and linguistic factors in the development of word
definitions. Journal of Child
Language, 15, 619-635.
Bogardus, E. S. (1949). Cultural pluralism and
acculturation. Sociology and Social Research, 34, 125-129.
Bogardus, E. S. (1950). Intercultural education and
acculturation. Sociology and Social Research, 34,
203-208.
Bossone, R. M. (1970). Disadvantaged teachers in
disadvantaged schools. Contemporary Education, 41, 183-185.
Bowen, M. (1978). Family practice in clinical practice.
New York: Jason Aronson.
Bronstein, P. (1986). Self-disclosure, paranoia, and
unaware racism: Another look at the black client and the
white therapist. American
Psychologist, 41, 225-226.
Brookover, W., Erickson, E., & Joiner, L.M. (1967).
Self-concept
of ability and school
achievement. III: Final report of
cooperative research
project N. 2831, Michigan State
University.
Buchheimer, A. (1968). A review of Hellmuth, J. (ed.) The
Disadvantaged Child. Seattle: Special Child
Publications, 1967. Personnel
and Guidance Journal, 46, 923-925.
Cole, S. G. (1946). The intercultural workshop. Sociology
and Social Research, 30, 476-483.
34
Congreve, W. J. (April, 1966). Not all the disadvantaged
are poor. The Education Digest, 31, 4-6.
Epps, E. G. (1969). Correlates of academic achievement
among northern and southern urban Negro students.
Journal of Social Issues, 25, 55-13.
Franklin, J. H. (1967). From Slavery to Freedom (3rd
ed.). Near York: Vintage Press.
Glatt, C. A. (November, 1965). Who are the deprived
children? The Education Digest, 31, 5-8.
Glazier, N. & Moynihan, D. P. (1963). Beyond the
melting pot: The Negroes. Puerto Ricans, Jews, Italians, and
Irish of New York City.
Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.
Grier, W., & Cobb, P. (1968). Black Rage. New
York: Bantam,
Griffith, A. R. (1978). A cultural perspective for
counseling blacks. Journal of College Student Personnel,
19, 249
Harper, F. D. (1973). What counselors must know about the
social sciences of Black Americans. Journal of
Negro Education, 42,
109-116.
Hawk, J. (1965). A comparison of academic and social
factors of culturally deprived children attending
deprived and non deprived
schools. Unpublished Master's thesis. Knoxville: Tennessee,
University of Tennessee.
Hawk, T. L. (1967). Self concept of the socially
disadvantaged. The Elementary School Journal, 67, 196-206.
35
Henkin, W. A. (1985). Toward counseling the Japanese in
America: A cross-cultural primer. Journal of
Counseling and Development, 63,
500-503.
Henson, K. T. (1973). Who are the disadvantaged? Clearing
House, 48, 117-120.
Higa, G. (1977). Ethnic scholarship and the 14th Amendment.
Journal of College Student Personnel, 18,
511-517.
Keneshige, E. (1973). Cultural factors in group counseling
and interaction. Personnel and Guidance Journal,
51, 407-412.
Kluckhohn, C. (1962). The concept of culture. In
Kluckhohn, R. Culture and Behavior. New York: The Free Press
of Glencoe.
Lipton, A. (1962). Cultural deprivation. The Journal of
Educational Sociology, 36,17-19.
Littlefield, R. P. (1974). Self-disclosure among
some Negro, White, and Mexican-American adolescents. Journal
of Counseling Psychology, 21,
133-136.
Myrdal, G. (1944). An American Dilemma. New York:
Harper.
Ornstein, A. C. (1966). Who are the disadvantaged? Journal
of Secondary Education, 41, 154-161.
Ornstein, A. C., & Levine, D. U. (1982). Multicultural
education: Trends and issues. Childhood Education, 58,
241-245.
Pate, G. S., & Garcia, J. (1981). Multi-ethnic/multi-cultural
education: A review of program. Clearing House,
55, 132-133.
36
Potkay, C., & Fullerton, J. (1973). Student perception
of pressures, helps, and psychological services.
Journal of College Student
Personnel, 14, 355-361.
Radke, M., Trager, H., & Davis, H. (1949). Social
perceptions and attitudes of children. Genetic Psychology
Monographs, 40,
360-367.
Radke-Yarrow, M., Trager, H., & Miller, J.
(1952). The role of parents in the development of children's
ethnic attitudes. Child
Development, 23, 13-53.
Report of the National Advisory Commission on Civil
Disorders. (1968). New York: Grossett.
Ridley, C. R. (1984). Clinical treatment of the
nondisclosing black client. American Psychologist, 39,
1234-1244.
Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Self-fulfilling
prophecies in the classroom: Teachers' expectations as
unintended determinants of
pupils' intellectual competence. In Deutsch, M., Katz, I., & Jensen, A.R. (eds.)
Social Class, Race, and Psychological Development. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and
Winston.
Ruiz, R., & Padilla, A. M. (1977). Counseling Latinos.
Personnel and Guidance Journal, 55, 401-408.
Scanzoni, J. J. (1970). Opportunity and the family.
New York: The Free Press.
Schmedinghoff, G. J. (1977). Counseling black students in
higher education: Is it racial, socioeconomic, or
human question? Journal of
College Student Personnel, 18, 472-477.
37
Sedlacek, W. E., & Brooks, G. C., Jr. (1970). Black
freshmen in large colleges: A survey. Personnel and
Guidance Journal, 39,
307-312.
Sedlacek, W. E., & Brooks, G. C., Jr. (1976). Racism
in American Education: A model for change, Chicago:
Nelson-Hall.
Seligman, L. (1977). Haitians: A neglected minority. Personnel
and Guidance Journal, 55, 409-411.
Sue, D. W. & Sue, D. (1973). Understanding
Asian-Americans: The neglected minority--An overview. Personnel
and Guidance Journal, 51,
387-389.
Thompson, C.E., Neville, H., Weathers, P.L., Poston, W.C.,
& Atkinson, D.R. (1990). Cultural mistrust and
racism reaction among African-American
and White-American. Journal of College Student Development, 31,
162-168.
Tracey, T. J., & Sedlacek, W. E. (1987). Prediction of
college graduation using noncognitive variables by
race. Measurement and
Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 19, 177-184.
Vela, J.E. (1977). A comparison of chicano and anglo
perceptions of the university environment. Journal of
College Student Personnel, 18,
462-466.
Watanabe, C. (1973). Self-expression and the Asian-American
experience. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 51,
394-402.
Weiner, M., & Murray, W. (1963). Another look at the
culturally deprived and their levels of aspiration. The Journal of Educational
Sociology, 36, 319-321.
38
Westbrook, F. D., Miyares, J., & Roberts, J. H. (1978).
Perceived problem areas by Black and White students
and hints about comparative
counseling needs. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 25, 131-136.
Westbrook, F. D., & Sedlacek, W. E. (1988). Workshop
on using non-cognitive variables with minority students
in higher education. Journal
for Specialists in Group Work, 13, 8289.
Westbrook, F. D. & Smith, J. B. (1976). Assisting
Black resident students at a predominantly White
institution: a paraprofessional
approach. Journal of College Student Personnel, 17, 205-210.
Westwood, M. J., & Ishiyama, F. I. (1990) The
communication process as a critical intervention for client
change in cross-cultural
counseling. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development. 18, 163-171.
Woodson, C. G., & Wesley, C. H. (1966). The Negro
in our history (11th Edition). Washington, D.C.: The
Associated Publishers.
Table 1: Descriptive Terminology for Articles
Published between January 1, 1950 and December 31, 1989 |
||||
Dates |
1950-59 |
1960-69 |
1970-79 |
1980-89 |
Mean Per Year |
||||
Journals Indexed |
185.4 |
291.2 |
233.6 |
350.9 |
Terms |
|
|
|
|
Acculturation |
2.5 |
1.6 |
0.1 |
0.7 |
Assimilation |
|
|
1.5 |
0.8 |
Intercultural |
|
|
|
|
Education |
15.6 |
10.2 |
4.6 |
0 |
Intergroup |
|
|
1.3 |
0 |
Intergroup |
|
|
|
|
Education |
- |
2.5 |
17.9 |
0 |
Socialization |
|
2 |
0.2 |
0 |
Cuturally |
|
|
|
|
Different |
- |
0.6 |
0 |
0 |
Multicultural |
|
|
|
|
Education |
- |
- |
75.5 |
44.7 |
Cross Cultural |
|
|
|
|
Studies |
- |
2.5 |
25.4 |
31.1 |
Cultural |
|
|
|
|
Differences |
- |
15.8 |
16.4 |
18.7 |
Ethnicity |
- |
- |
- |
5.4 |
Cultural |
|
|
|
|
Deprivation |
- |
23.9 |
0 |
0 |
Culturally |
|
|
|
|
Deprived |
- |
26 |
15.5 |
0.3 |
Culturally |
|
|
|
|
Deprived |
|
|
|
|
Counseling |
|
|
|
|
Services |
- |
2 |
18.2 |
17.6 |
Culturally |
|
|
|
|
Deprived |
|
|
|
|
Education |
- |
21.5 |
60.1 |
0 |
Teachers for |
|
|
|
|
the C.D. |
- |
61.5 |
10.2 |
4 |
Education for |
|
|
|
|
the T.C.D. |
- |
- |
10.9 |
9.8 |
Inservice |
|
|
|
|
T.C.D. |
- |
- |
3.7 |
0 |
Education for |
|
|
|
|
I.T.C.D. |
- |
- |
1 |
0 |
Counselor |
|
|
|
|
Education |
- |
- |
- |
53.6 |
Culturally |
|
|
|
|
Deprived |
|
|
|
|
Children |
- |
187.5 |
21.9 |
9.2 |
Education for |
|
|
|
|
C.D.C. |
- |
- |
55.4 |
1.8 |
Psychology |
- |
- |
- |
1.2 |
Racially |
|
|
|
|
Different |
4.5 |
0.8 |
0 |
0 |
Race |
|
|
|
|
Discrimination |
0.4 |
2.3 |
4.3 |
1.8 |
Race Prejudice |
4.5 |
4.8 |
3.7 |
1.2 |
Race Relations |
6.5 |
11.7 |
24.8 |
18.9 |
Racism |
- |
2.5 |
7.1 |
7.5 |
-
= Years before articles were indexed under term.
0 = No articles were indexed
under the term.
C.D.=Culturally Deprived
T.C.D.=Teachers of the
Culturally Deprived
I.T.C.D.=Inservice Teachers of
the Culturally Deprived
C.D.C=Culturally Deprived
Children
Table 2: Numbers and Percentages of Articles That
Were Indexed under Cross Cultural Studies with Foreign Country Names in the
Titles |
|||
|
|
Named |
|
|
|
Foreign |
|
Dates |
Articles |
Country in Title |
Percentages |
1968-69 |
24 |
10 |
42 |
1970-79 |
249 |
164 |
66 |
1980-89 |
319 |
192 |
60 |