University of Maryland
College Park, MD
Mapping the University Learning
Environment
Alice A. Mitchell
Marie T. Sergent
William E. Sedlacek
Research Report #14-94
Computer
time was provided by the Computer Science Center,
University of Maryland at College
Park.
COUNSELING CENTER
UNIVERSITY OF MARYLAND
COLLEGE PARK, MARYLAND
Mapping the University Learning
Environment
Alice A. Mitchell, Marie T. Sergent, and William E. Sedlacek
Research Report x#14-94
Mitchell, A. A., Sergent, M. T., & Sedlacek,
W. E.(1997).
Mapping the university learning
environment.
National Association
of Student Personnel Administrators Journal, 35, 20-28
Summary
Environmental assessment has long been a topic of professional interest. Perceptual mapping is an evolutionary step in campus ecology. The perceptual mapping technique provides respondents with a map of an indoor or outdoor location and their feelings or perceptions about areas on the map are solicited. This report explores the use of the perceptual mapping technique, demonstrating its use in examining campus perceptions of African American and White students.
Chi-square
analyses showed a significant difference in number of visits to campus by White
and African American students, with White students having visited more often.
More African American than White students named a recreational area as a
familiar location while more White students than African American students
named the student union; the differences were not statistically significant.
Data
display techniques, using graphics software, are illustrated for use in
presentations.
Introduction
Environmental
assessment has long been a topic of professional interest. Banning (1978)
termed this an "ecologic" trend and suggested the phrase "campus
ecology" to denote interest in college students and their interactions
with the campus environment. In campus ecology one seeks to design the campus
to meet the needs of its constituents, rather than to create structures through
which students and others might better fit the environment (Huebner, 1989).
This
interest in campus environments led to the development of several measurement
instruments, among them the College and University Environment Scales (CUES;
Pace, 1969), and the University Residence Environment Scales (URES; Moos & Gerst, 1974). In many instances, the environment (e.g.
residence hall) of interest was identified for the student respondent and
reactions to the location solicited.
While
these assessments have been helpful, perceptual mapping (Sergent
& Sedlacek, 1989) is an evolutionary step in
campus ecology. The perceptual mapping technique, further detailed below,
provides respondents with a map of an indoor or outdoor location and their
feelings or perceptions about areas on the map are solicited.
Correlates
of Environmental Perception
Student
perceptions of their environment have been linked to a host of attributes,
including satisfaction with college (Witt & Handal,
1984), student performance (Bauer, 1975), and stress
2
(Cohen & Wills, 1985).
Of more
recent interest have been studies which focus on student race and ethnicity in
an examination of campus environments. Numerous investigations have shown that
majority White institutions are perceived differently by students of color (de Armas & McDavis, 1981), and
sometimes as unwelcoming and hostile (Sedlacek, 1987;
Fleming, 1988). As our colleges and universities become increasingly diverse,
it is wise to better understand the environmental perceptions of our changed
student body. The purpose of this article is to explore the use of the
perceptual mapping technique, demonstrating its use in examining campus
perceptions of African American and White students.
Perceptual
Mapping Technique
As
mentioned above, the perceptual mapping approach allows respondents to indicate
locations of interest to them and their perceptions of those areas. The unique
aspect of perceptual mapping is that respondents use actual maps of interior or
exterior spaces and project feelings and perceptions on the maps. While there
are many environments in which the technique might be used, the present study
reports on its use at a university campus.
In the
first phase of the technique reported here, students were asked to indicate the
areas with which they were most familiar, the areas they intended to use, and
where they were most comfortable. General campus perceptions were also
obtained. In the second phase, students described how they felt about the
3
identified location by using 20 semantic differential paired adjectives (Osgood,
Suci, & Tannenbaum,
1957). These adjectives included such pairs as "friendly-hostile",
"warm-cold", and "pleasant-unpleasant".
Investigation
During
the regular freshman orientation program, students were given the Campus
Mapping Questionnaire (CMQ) on randomly selected days. More than 90% of all new
freshmen attend orientation and 100% participation was achieved on the days
selected. The CMQ consisted of a map of the campus and an attached survey which
sought demographic information and student identified locations. These
locations included the (1) building or area [location] with which the student
most wanted to become familiar, (2) the location the student believed he or she
would use most, (3) the location with which the student was most comfortable at
the present, and (4) the student's current perception of the entire campus.
Students
first indicated which location corresponded to each of the four descriptions by
marking that location on the map. For each of the four locations, students were
presented with twenty paired adjectives with which to describe the location.
The adjectives were the same for all four locations. On a five point Likert scale for each adjective pair, students indicated
the degree to which each location could be described on a bipolar continuum.
Adjective pairs included friendly-hostile, ugly-beautiful, and
meaningless-meaningful. After the surveys were
4
completed,
location responses were coded by the researchers as (1) library, (2) student
union, (3) recreation and leisure, (4) administrative, (5) classroom, (6)
counseling center, (7) chapel, (8) living areas, (9) open areas, or (0) other.
Results
Of the
411 respondents, 73% were White, 10% were African American, 14% were Asian
American, 1.2% were Hispanic, and 1.7% were members of
other racial and ethnic groups. The responses of African American and White
students were the focus of further analysis.
Previous
visits
Chi-square
analyses showed that White students had made significantly (.05 level) higher
visits to campus than had African American students (x2 = 9.95, df = 4, p = .04) when evaluated at
the .05 level. Most African American respondents (73%) had visited the campus
from one to five times, including the present visit. Only three African
American students (8%) had visited the campus more than 20 times. Sixty-three
percent of White respondents had visited from one to five times, with another
17% having visited from between 6 to ten times. Twelve percent of the White
respondents had visited more than 20 times.
While
White students had a chance to clarify their perception of the campus through
multiple visits, African American students were less likely to have had this
opportunity. Thus, for African American students especially those matriculating
from a predominantly African American environment,
5
this
predominantly White institution might still have had an aura of unfamiliarity.
White students might easily have felt more comfortable with the university
environment than African American students; it was predominantly White and they
had been there many times.
Familiar
locations
When
asked with which building or area they would most like to become familiar, the
greatest percentage, 40%, of the African American students named a recreational
area while 38% of the White students named the student union. Chi square analysis
of these two location choices by race (African American and White) showed
non-significant differences in the degree to which African American and White
students chose one location over the other (x2 = 1.75, df
= 1, p = .18).
Most
used locations
The
largest percentage (26%) of White students named the student union as the
location they would use the most. African Americans named both the student
union (23%) and classrooms (23%). Chi square analysis showed no significant
differences in the degree to which African American and White students chose
the student union or classroom as the place they anticipated using most (x2 =
.007, df = 1, p = .93). In research conducted by
Webster and Sedlacek (1982), 14% of their respondents
spent most of their time between classes in the student union. Of these, a
greater proportion of African American undergraduates (21%) spent time between
classes in the union than did White undergraduates.
6
Comfortable
locations
African
American students named the library as the area that was the most comfortable
for them (48%), with the classroom the next most comfortable (20%). For White
students, the student union was their most comfortable location (30%) with the
classroom as the next most comfortable location (21%). Chi square analysis
showed no significant differences in these choices (x Z = 2.38, df = 2, p = .30).
Data
display techniques
Since
perceptual mapping as a technique relies on a visual presentation of the
environment under study, it is appropriate that display of the resultant data
also use a more visual presentation medium.
The
advent of graphics software packages, particularly for microcomputer usage,
offers a rich array of possibilities. Such displays can assist researchers in
making their findings more immediately palatable to an audience with an applied
orientation. Figures 1 and 2 were created using the Microsoft EXCEL
"radar" figure, produced on transparencies for presentation. The
figures show student answers to the question asking them to name their most
comfortable place.
The
response of African American and White students who named classroom areas as
their most comfortable are compared. Each of the three axes represents a
semantic-differential item, (1) friendly-hostile, (2) warm-cold, and (3) pleasant-unpleasant.
In this
particular example, no significant differences were
7
found
in African American and White responses (t-test on friendly-hostile yielded t =
-1.11, df = 68, p = .27; t-test on warm-cold: t =
-.30, df = 68, p = .76; t-test on pleasant
unpleasant: t = -.46, df = 68, p = .65. All results
were evaluated at .05, divided by 3 as a Bonferroni
correction). Thus the figures are for example only,
rather than to show systematic perceptual differences. In the
diagrams, African American
students
found the classroom to be more friendly, warm, and pleasant than did White
students, but, again, not to significant levels. Superimposing one transparency
over the other shows this difference and should be readily apparent to an
audience.
Discussion
Perceptual mapping, used with new students, identified areas of
interest and initial perceptions. These perceptions, and subsequent reality, may have
important ramifications for retention of African American students at
predominantly White institutions.
For new
African American students, the campus is one which is less familiar than it is
for White students. This lower familiarity may have important relationships to
retention, where comfortability is strongly related
to persistence.
A predominantly-White
campus concerned with the degree to which students of color find a campus
attractive might examine the results of perceptual mapping. In the study
reported above, it is interesting that no African American student indicated
the chapel as an area with which she or he wished to become familiar.
8 In view
of the importance of spiritual values in African American culture (McEwen,
Roper, Bryant, & Langa, 1989), the absence of a
religious contact or reference point on campus is one of several potentially
alienating factors. Astin (1973) and Lea, Sedlacek, and Stewart (1979) showed the value of having a
contact point in retention.
Also
noteworthy for its absence was the Black Cultural Center on campus. This
center, located within one of the dining hall buildings, was not discernibly
mentioned in any survey. Campus administrators in orientation and admissions
may wish to include these and other important contact points in their work with
African American students.
Perceptual
mapping can be used in a wide variety of environments in addition to the
university campus. Additional applications might include surveying the
perceptions of building employees in a corporate setting when deciding where to
locate a new facility. If the facility were designed to stimulate interaction,
it might be located in an area already perceived as welcoming to those it might
serve. In another application, customer service areas could be surveyed not
only for the efficiency of services rendered but for the perception created for
those who are served.
In the
university setting, perceptual mapping can provide a way to evaluate a campus
through the eyes of students. Communicating the results of such evaluations to
administrators who can effect change in the environment is of paramount
9
importance. Visual techniques appropriate to the investigation may be helpful in
making research results more consumer-friendly. As we seek better ways of
serving our diverse student clientele, a clearer understanding of the
perception of our services and locations will inform our efforts.
10
References
Astin,
A. W. (1973). Impact of dorm living on students. Educational
Record, 54, 204-210.
Banning, J. H. (1978). Campus ecology: A perspective for student affairs.
Cincinnati, OH: National Association of Student Personnel Administrators.
Bauer, G.
E. (1975). Performance and satisfaction as a function of person-environment
fit. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Missouri, Columbia.
Cohen, S. & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social
support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin,
98, 310-357.
De Armas, C. P. & McDavis,
R. J. (1981). White, Black, and Hispanic students' perceptions of a community
college environment. Journal of College Student
Personnel, 22, 33 341.
Fleming,
J. (1988). Blacks in college. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Huebner,
L. A. (1989). Interaction of student and campus. In U. Delworth & G. R. Hanson (Eds.).
Student services: A handbook for the profession. 2nd Ed. (pp. 165-208).
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Lea, D. H., Sedlacek, W. E. & Stewart,
S. S. (1979).
Problems in retention research in higher education NASPA Journal, 17(1),
2-8.
11
McEwen, M. L., Roper, L. D., Bryant, D. R., & Langa,
M. J. (1989). Challenging the boundaries of psychosocial theories: Incorporating
the development of African-American students. Journal of College
Student Development, 30, 429-436.
Moos, R. H. & Gerst, M. S. (1974). University
residence environment scale. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist
Press.
Osgood, C., Suci, G., and Tannenbaum, P. (1957). The measurement of meaning., Urbana,
IL: University of Illinois Press.
Pace, C. R. (1969). College and university environment scale.
Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
Sedlacek,
W. S. (1987). Black students on White campuses: 20 years of research. Journal
of College Student Personnel, 28(6), 484-495.
Sergent, M. T. & Sedlacek,
W. E. (1989).
Perceptual mapping: A methodology in the assessment of environmental
perceptions. Journal of College Student Development, 30, 319-322.
Webster, D. W. and Sedlacek, W. E. (1982). The
differential impact of a university student union on campus subgroups. NASPA
Journal, 19(2), 48-51.
Witt, P.
and Handal, P. (1984) Person-environment fit: Is
satisfaction predicted by congruency, environment, or personality? Journal
of College Student Personnel, 25, 503-512.
Figure
Caption Figure 1. Boxed points would be printed in color for use in
overhead transparencies.